Jump to content

Shepard tone

From Niidae Wiki

Template:Short description Template:Use dmy dates

File:Shepard Tones spectrum linear scale.png
A spectrogram of ascending Shepard tones on a linear frequency scale

A Shepard tone, named after Roger Shepard, is a sound consisting of a superposition of sine waves separated by octaves. When played with the bass pitch of the tone moving upward or downward, it is referred to as the Shepard scale. This creates the auditory illusion of a tone that seems to continually ascend or descend in pitch, yet which ultimately gets no higher or lower.<ref name="Shepard1964" />

Construction

[edit]
File:Shepard tone.jpg
Figure 1: Shepard tones forming a Shepard scale, illustrated in a sequencer

Each square in Figure 1 indicates a tone, with any set of squares in vertical alignment together making one Shepard tone. The color of each square indicates the loudness of the note, with purple being the quietest and green the loudest. Overlapping notes that play at the same time are exactly one octave apart, and each scale fades in and fades out so that hearing the beginning or end of any given scale is impossible.

File:Shepard A.ogg
Shepard tone as of the root note A (A4 = 440 Hz)
File:Shepard scale diatonic C.ogg
Shepard scale, diatonic in C Major, repeated 5 times

As a conceptual example of an ascending Shepard scale, the first tone could be an almost inaudible C4 (middle C) and a loud C5 (an octave higher). The next would be a slightly louder CTemplate:Music4 and a slightly quieter CTemplate:Music5; the next would be a still louder D4 and a still quieter D5. The two frequencies would be equally loud at the middle of the octave (FTemplate:Music4 and FTemplate:Music5), and the twelfth tone would be a loud B4 and an almost inaudible B5 with the addition of an almost inaudible B3. The thirteenth tone would then be the same as the first, and the cycle could continue indefinitely. (In other words, each tone consists of two sine waves with frequencies separated by octaves; the intensity of each is e.g. a raised cosine function of its separation in semitones from a peak frequency, which in the above example would be B4. According to Shepard, "almost any smooth distribution that tapers off to subthreshold levels at low and high frequencies would have done as well as the cosine curve actually employed."<ref name="Shepard1964">Template:Cite journal</ref>

The theory behind the illusion was demonstrated during an episode of the BBC's show Bang Goes the Theory, where the effect was described as "a musical barber's pole".<ref>Template:Cite episode</ref>

The scale as described, with discrete steps between each tone, is known as the discrete Shepard scale. The illusion is more convincing if there is a short time between successive notes (staccato or marcato rather than legato or portamento).Template:Citation needed

Variants

[edit]
File:Shepard Tone spectrum video visualization.webm
Moving audio and video visualization of a rising Shepard–Risset glissando. See and hear the higher tones as they fade out.

Shepard–Risset glissando

[edit]

Jean-Claude Risset subsequently created a version of the scale where the tones glide continuously, and it is appropriately called the continuous Risset scale or Shepard–Risset glissando.<ref name="CDM">Template:Cite news</ref> When done correctly, the tone appears to rise (or fall) continuously in pitch, yet return to its starting note. Risset has also created a similar effect with rhythm in which tempo seems to increase or decrease endlessly.<ref>Template:Cite web</ref>

File:Risset accelerando beat1 MCLD.ogg
An example of Risset's accelerating rhythm effect using a breakbeat loop

Tritone paradox

[edit]

Template:Main A sequentially played pair of Shepard tones separated by an interval of a tritone (half an octave) produces the tritone paradox. Shepard had predicted that the two tones would constitute a bistable figure, the auditory equivalent of the Necker cube, that could be heard ascending or descending, but never both at the same time.<ref name="Shepard1964" />

File:Paradoxa del tríton.wav
Sequence of Shepard tones producing the tritone paradox

In 1986, Diana Deutsch discovered that the perception of which tone was higher depended on the absolute frequencies involved and that an individual would usually hear the same pitch as the highest (this is determined by the absolute pitch of the notes).<ref name="Deutsch1986">Template:Cite journal</ref> Interestingly, different listeners may perceive the same pattern as being either ascending or descending, depending on the language or dialect of the listener (Deutsch, Henthorn, and Dolson found that native speakers of Vietnamese, a tonal language, heard the tritone paradox differently from Californians who were native speakers of English).<ref name="Deutsch1992">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Perpetual melody

[edit]

Pedro Patricio observed in 2012 that, by using a Shepard tone as a sound source and applying it to a melody, he could reproduce the illusion of a continuously ascending or descending movement characteristic of the Shepard Scale. Regardless of the tempo and the envelope of the notes, the auditory illusion is effectively maintained. The uncertainty of the scale the Shepard tones pertain allows composers to experiment with deceiving and disconcerting melodies.<ref>Patricio, Pedro. From the Shepard tone to the perpetual melody auditory illusion. Proceedings of the 9th Sound and Music Computing Conference, SMC 2012. 5-10, 2012.</ref>

File:Melodia Perpètua.wav
An example of an ascendent perpetual melody

Template:Clear

Examples

[edit]

See also

[edit]

Template:Portal

References

[edit]

Template:Reflist

[edit]

Template:Commons category

Template:Auditory illusions Template:Music psychology Template:Music technology