Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Psychophysiology
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Branch of psychology}} {{distinguish|Physiological psychology}} {{For |the journal |Psychophysiology (journal){{!}}''Psychophysiology'' (journal)}} {{Use American English|date=July 2023}} {{Cleanup|date=April 2011}} '''Psychophysiology''' (from [[Ancient Greek|Greek]] {{lang|grc|ฯแฟกฯฮฎ}}, ''psศณkhฤ'', "breath, life, soul"; {{lang|grc|ฯฯฯฮนฯ}}, ''physis'', "nature, origin"; and {{lang|grc|-ฮปฮฟฮณฮฏฮฑ}}, ''[[wiktionary:-logia|-logia]]'') is the branch of [[psychology]] that is concerned with the [[physiology|physiological]] bases of [[psychology|psychological]] processes.<ref>{{MeshName|Psychophysiology}}</ref> While psychophysiology was a general, broad field of research in the 1960s and 1970s, it has now become quite specialized, based on methods, topic of studies, and scientific traditions. Methods vary as combinations of [[Electrophysiology|electrophysiological]] methods (such as [[EEG]]), [[neuroimaging]] ([[MRI]], [[Positron emission tomography|PET]]), and [[neurochemistry]]. Topics have branched into subspecializations such as social, sport, cognitive, cardiovascular, clinical, and other branches of psychophysiology. == Background == {{unreferenced section|date=August 2016}} Some people have difficulty distinguishing a psychophysiologist from a [[Physiological psychology|physiological psychologist]], which has two very different perspectives. [[Psychologist]]s are interested in why we may fear [[spider]]s and physiologists may be interested in the [[input/output]] system of the [[amygdala]]. A psychophysiologist will attempt to link the two.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Cacioppo |first1=John T. |last2=Tassinary |first2=Louis G. |date=1990 |title=Inferring psychological significance from physiological signals. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0003-066X.45.1.16 |journal=American Psychologist |language=en |volume=45 |issue=1 |pages=16โ28 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.45.1.16 |pmid=2297166 |issn=1935-990X}}</ref> Psychophysiologists generally study the psychological/physiological link in intact human subjects. While early psychophysiologists almost always examined the impact of psychological states on physiological system responses, since the 1970s, psychophysiologists have also frequently studied the impact of physiological states and systems on psychological states and processes. It is this perspective of studying the interface of mind and body that makes psychophysiologists most distinct.<ref>{{Citation |last1=Schell |first1=A. |title=Psychophysiology |date=2001-01-01 |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B0080430767034240 |encyclopedia=International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences |pages=12448โ12452 |editor-last=Smelser |editor-first=Neil J. |access-date=2023-12-14 |place=Oxford |publisher=Pergamon |doi=10.1016/b0-08-043076-7/03424-0 |isbn=978-0-08-043076-8 |last2=Dawson |first2=M. E. |editor2-last=Baltes |editor2-first=Paul B.}}</ref> Historically, most psychophysiologists tended to examine the physiological responses and [[Biological system|organ systems]] innervated by the [[autonomic nervous system]]. More recently, psychophysiologists have been equally, or potentially more, interested in the [[central nervous system]], exploring [[Human brain#Cerebrum|cortical brain]] potentials such as the many types of [[event-related potentials]] (ERPs), [[Neural oscillation|brain waves]], and utilizing advanced technology such as [[functional magnetic resonance imaging]] (fMRI), [[Magnetic resonance imaging|MRI]], [[Positron emission tomography|PET]], MEG, and other neuroimagery techniques.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Critchley |first=Hugo D. |date=2009-08-01 |title=Psychophysiology of neural, cognitive and affective integration: fMRI and autonomic indicants |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0167876009001020 |journal=International Journal of Psychophysiology |series=Neural Processes in Clinical Psychophysiology |volume=73 |issue=2 |pages=88โ94 |doi=10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.01.012 |issn=0167-8760|pmc=2722714 }}</ref> A psychophysiologist may look at how exposure to a stressful situation will produce a result in the [[Circulatory system|cardiovascular system]] such as a change in [[heart rate]] (HR), [[vasodilation]]/[[vasoconstriction]], [[myocardial contractility]], or [[stroke volume]].{{Citation needed|date=September 2024}} Overlaps in areas of interest between psychophysiologists and physiological psychologist may consist of observing how one cardiovascular event may influence another cardiovascular or [[Endocrine system|endocrine]] event; or how activation of one neural brain structure exerts excitatory activity in another neural structure which then induces an inhibitory effect in some other system.{{Citation needed|date=September 2024}} Often, physiological psychologists examine the effects that they study in [[Infrahumanisation|infrahuman]] subjects using surgical or invasive techniques and processes.{{Citation needed|date=September 2024}} Psychophysiology is closely related to the field of [[neuroscience]], which primarily concerns itself with relationships between [[psychological]] events and [[Human brain|brain]] processes. Psychophysiology is also related to the medical disciplines, such as [[endocrinology]], [[psychosomatic]], and [[psychopharmacology]].{{Citation needed|date=September 2024}} While psychophysiology was a discipline off the mainstream of psychological and medical science prior to roughly the 1940s, more recently, psychophysiology has found itself positioned at the intersection of psychological and medical science, and its popularity and importance have expanded commensurately with the realization of the inter-relatedness of mind and body.{{Citation needed|date=September 2024}} ==Measures== Psychophysiology measures exist in multiple domains; reports, electrophysiological studies, studies in [[neurochemistry]], neuroimaging, and behavioral methods.<ref name=":0">{{cite book|last1=Cacioppo|first1=John|last2=Tassinary|first2=Louis|last3=Berntson|first3=Gary|title=Handbook of Psychophysiology|date=2007|publisher=Cambridge University Press|pages=581โ607|edition=3rd|chapter=25}}</ref> Evaluative reports involve participant introspection and self-ratings of internal psychological states or physiological sensations, such as self-report of arousal levels on the [[Self-Assessment Manikin|self-assessment manikin]],<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Bradley|first1=Margaret|last2=Lang|first2=Peter|title=Measuring Emotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential|journal=Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry|date=1994|volume=25|issue=1|pages=49โ59|doi=10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9|pmid=7962581|s2cid=17630161 }}</ref> or measures of interoceptive visceral awareness such as heartbeat detection.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Weins|first1=Stefan|last2=Mezzacappa|first2=Elizabeth|last3=Katkin|first3=Edward|title=Heartbeat Detection and the Experience of Emotions|journal=Cognition and Emotion|date=2000|volume=14|issue=3|pages=417โ427|doi=10.1080/026999300378905|s2cid=14815385}}</ref> Merits to self-report are an emphasis on accurately understand the participants' subjective experience and understanding their perception; however, its pitfalls include the possibility of participants misunderstanding a scale or incorrectly recalling events.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Robinson|first1=Michael|last2=Clore|first2=Gerald|title=Episodic and Semantic Knowledge in Emotional Self-Report: Evidence for Two Judgment Processes|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|date=2002|volume=83|issue=1|pages=198โ215|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.198|pmid=12088126|s2cid=17805222 }}</ref> Physiological responses also can be measured via instruments that read bodily events such as heart rate change, [[electrodermal activity|electrodermal activity (EDA)]], muscle tension, and cardiac output.<ref>{{Citation |last1=Tassinary |first1=Louis G. |title=The perimetric physiological measurement of psychological constructs. |date=2023 |url=https://doi.org/10.1037/0000318-025 |work=APA handbook of research methods in psychology: Foundations, planning, measures, and psychometrics (Vol. 1) (2nd ed.). |pages=531โ564 |access-date=2023-12-21 |place=Washington |publisher=American Psychological Association |language=en |doi=10.1037/0000318-025 |isbn=978-1-4338-3713-5 |last2=Hess |first2=Ursula |last3=Carcoba |first3=Luis M. |last4=Orr |first4=Joseph M.}}</ref> Many indices are part of modern psychophysiology, including brain waves (electroencephalography, EEG), [[fMRI]] (functional magnetic resonance imaging), electrodermal activity (a standardized term encompassing skin conductance response, SCR, and galvanic skin response, GSR), cardiovascular measures ([[heart rate]], HR; [[beats per minute]], BPM; [[heart rate variability]], HRV; vasomotor activity), muscle activity ([[electromyography]], EMG), [[Electrogastrogram|electrogastrogram (EGG)]] changes in pupil diameter with thought and emotion ([[pupillometry]]), eye movements, recorded via the electro-oculogram (EOG) and direction-of-gaze methods, cardiodynamics, recorded via [[impedance cardiography]], and grip force.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Sahar |first1=Yotam |last2=Elbaum |first2=Tomer |last3=Musicant |first3=Oren |last4=Wagner |first4=Michael |last5=Altarac |first5=Leon |last6=Shoval |first6=Shraga |date=2023-02-23 |title=Mapping Grip Force Characteristics in the Measurement of Stress in Driving |journal=International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health |language=en |volume=20 |issue=5 |pages=4005 |doi=10.3390/ijerph20054005 |issn=1660-4601 |pmc=10002433 |pmid=36901016 |doi-access=free }}</ref> These measures are beneficial because they provide accurate and perceiver-independent objective data recorded by machinery.<ref name=":0" /> The downsides, however, are that any physical activity or motion can alter responses, and basal levels of arousal and responsiveness can differ among individuals and even between situations.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Glynn|first1=Laura|last2=Christenfeld|first2=Nicholas|last3=Gerin|first3=William|title=The Role of Rumination in Recovery From Reactivity; Cardiovascular Consequences of Emotional States|journal=Psychosomatic Medicine|date=2002|volume=64|issue=5|pages=714โ726|doi=10.1097/01.psy.0000031574.42041.23|pmid=12271102|s2cid=1533394|doi-access=free}}</ref> Neurochemical methods are used to study functionality and processes associated to [[neurotransmitters]] and [[neuropeptides]]<ref>{{cite book|last1 = Brady| first1 = ST |author2=Siegel GJ, Albers RW, Price DL.| year = 2012|title = Basic Neurochemistry. McGill Press}}</ref> Finally, one can measure overt action or behavior, which involves the observation and recording actual actions, such as running, freezing, eye movement, and facial expression. These are good response measures and easy to record in animals, but they are not as frequently used in human studies.<ref name=":0" /> == Uses == Psychophysiological measures are often used to study [[emotion]] and attention responses to stimuli, during exertion, and increasingly, to better understand [[cognitive]] processes. Physiological sensors have been used to detect emotions in schools<ref>{{cite journal | last = Arroyo | first = Ivon |author2=Woolf, B |author3=Cooper, D |author4=Burleson, W |author5=Muldner, K |author6=Christopherson, R | year = 2009 | title = Emotion Sensors Go To School | journal = Artificial Intelligence in Education | volume = 1 | issue = 1 | pages = 18โ37 }}</ref> and [[intelligent tutoring systems]].<ref>{{cite book | last = Hussein | first = M.S |author2=Hussain, M. S. |author3=AlZoubi, O. |author4=Calvo, R. A. |author5=D'Mello, S. K. | year = 2011 | chapter=Affect Detection from Multichannel Physiology during Learning Sessions with Auto ''Tutor'' | title = Artificial Intelligence in Education | series = Lecture Notes in Computer Science | volume = 6738 | pages = 131โ138 |doi=10.1007/978-3-642-21869-9_19 | isbn = 978-3-642-21868-2 }}</ref> ==Emotions as example of psychophysiological studies== {{ced|section|for=proper paragraphing. Excessively long, run-on paragraph needs to be split into multiple paragraphs of moderate length|date=September 2024}} Psychophysiology studies multiple aspects of behavior, and emotions are the most common example. It has long been recognized that emotional episodes are partly constituted by physiological responses.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Williams|first1=James|title=What is an Emotion?|journal=Mind|date=1884|volume=34|issue=2|pages=188โ205}}</ref> Early work done linking emotions to psychophysiology started with research on mapping consistent autonomic nervous system (ANS) responses to discrete emotional states. For example, anger might be constituted by a certain set of physiological responses, such as increased cardiac output and high diastolic blood pressure, which would allow us to better understand patterns and predict emotional responses. Some studies were able to detect consistent patterns of ANS responses that corresponded to specific emotions under certain contexts, like an early study by Paul Ekman and colleagues in 1983 "Emotion-specific activity in the autonomic nervous system was generated by constructing facial prototypes of emotion muscle by muscle and by reliving past emotional experiences. The autonomic activity produced distinguished not only between positive and negative emotions, but also among negative emotions".<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Paul|first1=Ekman|last2=Levenson|first2=Robert|last3=Friesen|first3=Wallace|title=Autonomic Nervous System Activity Distinguishes Among Emotions|journal=Science|date=1983|volume=221|issue=4616|pages=1208โ1210|doi=10.1126/science.6612338|pmid=6612338|bibcode=1983Sci...221.1208E|s2cid=15285913 }}</ref> However, as more studies were conducted, more variability was found in ANS responses to discrete emotion inductions, not only among individuals but also over time in the same individuals, and greatly between social groups.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Cacioppo|first1=John|last2=Berntson|first2=Gary|last3=Larsen|first3=Jeff|last4=Poehlmann|first4=Kirsten|last5=Ito|first5=Tiffany|title=The Psychophysiology of Emotion|journal=Handbook of Emotions|date=2000|volume=2|pages=173โ191}}</ref> Some of these differences can be attributed to variables like induction technique, context of the study, or classification of stimuli, which can alter a perceived scenario or emotional response. However, it was also found that features of the participant could also alter ANS responses. Factors such as basal level of arousal at the time of experimentation or between-test recovery, learned or conditioned responses to certain stimuli, range and maximal level of effect of ANS action, and individual attentiveness can all alter physiological responses in a lab setting.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Stemmler|first1=Gerhard|last2=Wacker|first2=Jan|title=Personality, Emotion, and Individual Differences in Physiological Responses|journal=Biological Psychology|date=2010|volume=84|issue=3|pages=541โ551|doi=10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.012|pmid=19800934|s2cid=24408358}}</ref> Even supposedly discrete emotional states fail to show specificity. For example, some emotional typologists consider fear to have subtypes, which might involve fleeing or freezing, both of which can have distinct physiological patterns and potentially distinct neural circuitry.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gross|first1=Cornelius|last2=Canteras|first2=Newton|title=The Many Paths to Fear|journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience|date=2012|volume=13|issue=9|pages=651โ658|doi=10.1038/nrn3301|pmid=22850830|s2cid=5319555}}</ref> As such no definitive correlation can be drawn linking specific autonomic patterns to discrete emotions, causing emotion theorists to rethink classical definitions of emotions. == Psychophysiological inference and physiological computer games == Physiological computing represents a category of [[affective computing]] that incorporates real-time software adaption to the psychophysiological activity of the user. The main goal of this is to build a computer that responds to user emotion, cognition and motivation. The approach is to enable implicit and symmetrical human-computer communication by granting the software access to a representation of the user's psychological status. There are several possible methods to represent the psychological state of the user (discussed in the [[affective computing]] page). The advantages of using psychophysiological indices are that their changes are continuous, measures are covert and implicit, and only available data source when the user interacts with the computer without any explicit communication or input device. These systems rely upon an assumption that the psychophysiological measure is an accurate one-to-one representation of a relevant psychological dimension, such as mental effort, task engagement, and frustration. Physiological computing systems all contain an element that may be termed as an adaptive controller that may be used to represent the player. This adaptive controller represents the decision-making process underlying software adaptation. In their simplest form, adaptive controllers are expressed in [[Boolean expression|Boolean]] statements. Adaptive controllers encompass not only the decision-making rules but also the psychophysiological inference that is implicit in the quantification of those trigger points used to activate the rules. The representation of the player using an adaptive controller can become very complex and often only one-dimensional. The loop used to describe this process is known as the biocybernetic loop. The biocybernetic loop describes the closed-loop system that receives psychophysiological data from the player, transforms that data into a computerized response, which then shapes the future psychophysiological response from the player. A positive control loop tends towards instability as the player-software loop strives towards a higher standard of desirable performance. The physiological computer game may wish to incorporate both positive and negative loops into the adaptive controller.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Gruszynski|first=Mike|author2=Stephen H Faircloug |title=Psychophysiological Inference and Physiological Computer Games}}</ref> ==See also== *[[Karl U. Smith]] *[[Vladimir Nebylitsyn]] *[[Jemma B. King]] *[[Physiological psychology]] *[[Search activity concept]] *[[Behavior change (individual)|Behavior change]] == References == ===Citations=== {{Reflist|30em}} ===Bibliography=== {{Refbegin}} *{{cite journal | last1 = Bos | first1 = M. W. | last2 = Dijksterhuis | first2 = A. | last3 = Van Baaren | first3 = R. | year = 2012 | title = Food for thought? Trust your unconscious when energy is low | journal = Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics | volume = 5 | issue = 2| pages = 124โ130 | doi=10.1037/a0027388}} *{{cite journal | last1 = Cushman | first1 = F. | last2 = Gary | first2 = K. | last3 = Gaffey | first3 = A. | last4 = Mendes | first4 = W. B. | year = 2012 | title = Simulating murder: The aversion to harmful action | journal = Emotion | volume = 12 | issue = 1| pages = 2โ7 | doi=10.1037/a0025071| pmid = 21910540 }} *{{cite journal | last1 = Fabiani | first1 = M | year = 2012 | title = It was the best of times, it was the worst of times: A psychophysiologist's view of cognitive aging | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 49 | issue = 3| pages = 283โ304 | doi=10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01331.x| pmid = 22220910 | doi-access = free }} *{{cite journal | last1 = Greenland | first1 = K. | last2 = Xenias | first2 = D. | last3 = Maio | first3 = G. | year = 2012 | title = Intergroup anxiety from the self and other: Evidence from self-report, physiological effects, and real interactions | journal = European Journal of Social Psychology | volume = 42 | issue = 2| pages = 150โ163 | doi=10.1002/ejsp.867}} *{{cite journal | last1 = Kakarot | first1 = N. | last2 = Mueller | first2 = F. | last3 = Bassarak | first3 = C. | year = 2012 | title = Activityโrest schedules in physically demanding work and the variation of responses with age | journal = Ergonomics | volume = 55 | issue = 3| pages = 282โ294 | doi=10.1080/00140139.2011.639906| pmid = 22273280 | s2cid = 25508336 }} *{{cite journal | last1 = Kircanski | first1 = K. | last2 = Morazavi | first2 = A. | last3 = Castriotta | first3 = N. | last4 = Baker | first4 = A. S. | last5 = Mystkowski | first5 = J. L. | last6 = Yi | first6 = R. | last7 = Craske | first7 = M. G. | year = 2012 | title = Challenges to the traditional exposure paradigm: Variability in exposure therapy for contamination fears | journal = Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry | volume = 43 | issue = 2| pages = 745โ751 | doi=10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.10.010| pmid = 22104655 }} *{{cite journal | last1 = Ong | first1 = A. D. | last2 = Rothstein | first2 = J. D. | last3 = Uchino | first3 = B. N. | year = 2012 | title = Loneliness accentuates age differences in cardiovascular responses to social evaluate threat | journal = Psychology and Aging | volume = 27 | issue = 1| pages = 190โ198 | doi=10.1037/a0025570| pmid = 22004517 }} *{{cite journal | last1 = Pietschnig | first1 = J. | last2 = Nader | first2 = I. W. | last3 = Gittler | first3 = G. | year = 2012 | title = Pheromone exposure impairs spatial task performance in young men | journal = Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science | volume = 44 | issue = 2| pages = 93โ98 | doi=10.1037/a0026194}} *{{cite journal | last1 = Satpute | first1 = A. B. | last2 = Mumford | first2 = J. A. | last3 = Naliboff | first3 = B. D. | last4 = Poldrack | first4 = R. A. | year = 2012 | title = Human anterior and posterior hippocampus respond distinctly to state and trait anxiety | doi = 10.1037/a0026517 | pmid = 22309734 | journal = Emotion | volume = 12 | issue = 1| pages = 56โ68 }} *{{cite journal | last1 = Van Dooren | first1 = M. | last2 = de Vries | first2 = J. J. G. | last3 = Janssen | first3 = J. H. | year = 2012 | title = Emotional sweating across the body: Comparing 16 different skin conductance measurement locations | journal = Physiology and Behavior | volume = 106 | issue = 2| pages = 298โ304 | doi=10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.01.020| pmid = 22330325 | s2cid = 19704826 }} * Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology. Heart Rate Variability Standards of Measurement, Physiological Interpretation, and Clinical Use. ''Circulation''. 1996:1043-1065. * Heel-Lancing in Newborns: Behavioral and Spectral Analysis Assessment of Pain Control Methods. A. Weissman, M. Aranovitch, S. Blazer, and E. Z. Zimmer (2009) Pediatrics 124, e921-e92 * Effects of Low-Intensity Exercise Conditioning on Blood Pressure, Heart Rate, and Autonomic Modulation of Heart Rate in Men and Women with Hypertension. L. P.T. Hua, C. A. Brown, S. J.M. Hains, M. Godwin, and J. L. Parlow (2009) Biol Res Nurs 11, 129โ143 * Malik M, Camm A. ''Heart Rate Variability''. Futura Publishing Company, 1995. *{{citation |year=1990 |author=[[John Cacioppo|Cacioppo, John T.]], & Tassinary, Louis G |title=Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social, and inferential elements |place=Cambridge |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=978-0-521-34885-0 |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/principlesofpsyc0000unse }} *{{citation |year=2017 |author=[[John Cacioppo|Cacioppo, John T.]], Tassinary, Louis G. & Bernston, Gary |title=Handbook of psychophysiology 4th Edition |place=Cambridge |publisher=Cambridge University Press}} * Welcome MO, Pereverzeva EV, and Pereverzev VA. A novel psychophysiological model of the effect of alcohol use on academic performance of male medical students of Belarusian State Medical University. IJCRIMPH 2 (6): 183โ197, 2010. {{Refend}} ==External links== {{Commons category|Physiologists}} * [http://www.sprweb.org/ Society for Psychophysiological Research. The primary American professional organization of psychophysiological research.] * [http://www.the-bscp.com British Society for Clinical Psychophysiology (BSCP) Clinical Psychophysiology] * [https://web.archive.org/web/20011031181146/http://www.ohiou.edu/isarp/ The International Society for the Advancement of Respiratory Psychophysiology (ISARP)] * [https://web.archive.org/web/20070105015000/http://www.medipsych.co.uk/ The Medipsych Institute Clinical Psychophysiology] * [https://web.archive.org/web/20091024081211/http://www.eecs.tufts.edu/~agirou01/workshop/ Brain, Body and Bytes: Psychophysiological User Interaction] ''CHI 2010 Workshop'' (10โ15, April 2010) {{Library resources box |by=no |onlinebooks=no |others=no |about=yes |label=psychophysiology}} {{Psychophysiology}} {{Psychology}} {{Physiology types}} {{Authority control}} [[Category:Neuropsychology]]
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Templates used on this page:
Template:Authority control
(
edit
)
Template:Ced
(
edit
)
Template:Citation
(
edit
)
Template:Citation needed
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cleanup
(
edit
)
Template:Commons category
(
edit
)
Template:Distinguish
(
edit
)
Template:For
(
edit
)
Template:Lang
(
edit
)
Template:Library resources box
(
edit
)
Template:MeshName
(
edit
)
Template:Physiology types
(
edit
)
Template:Psychology
(
edit
)
Template:Psychophysiology
(
edit
)
Template:Refbegin
(
edit
)
Template:Refend
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Unreferenced section
(
edit
)
Template:Use American English
(
edit
)
Search
Search
Editing
Psychophysiology
Add topic