Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Müller-Lyer illusion
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{short description|Optical illusion}} {{cs1 config|mode=cs1|name-list-style=vanc|display-authors=6}} [[Image:Müller-Lyer_Illusion_-_MathWorld_version.svg|thumb|Two sets of arrows that exhibit the Müller-Lyer optical illusion. The set on the bottom represents the classic Müller-Lyer stimulus, while on the top is its [[Franz Brentano | Brentano]] modification. All the shafts of the arrows are of the same physical length.]] The '''Müller-Lyer illusion''' is an [[optical illusion]] consisting of three stylized arrows. When viewers are asked to place a mark on the figure at the midpoint, they tend to place it more towards the "tail" end. The illusion was devised by [[Franz Carl Müller-Lyer]] (1857–1916), a German sociologist, in 1889.<ref>{{cite journal| vauthors = Müller-Lyer FC |date=1889 |title=Optische Urteilstäuschungen | trans-title = Optical illusions |journal=Archiv für Physiologie Suppl. |volume=1889 |pages=263–270 |url=https://www.biodiversitylibrary.org/page/35372625}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal| vauthors = Brentano F |date=1892 |title=Über ein optisches Paradoxon | trans-title = About an optical paradox | language = de |journal=Zeitschrift für Psychologie |volume=3 |pages=349–358 |url=http://echo.mpiwg-berlin.mpg.de/MPIWG:793V9QMK}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal| vauthors = Müller-Lyer FC |date=1894 |title=Über Kontrast und Konfluxion | trans-title = On contrast and confusion | language = de |journal=Zeitschrift für Psychologie |volume=9 |pages=1–16}}</ref> A variation of the same effect (and the most common form in which it is seen today) consists of a set of arrow-like figures. Straight line segments of equal length comprise the "shafts" of the arrows, while shorter line segments (called the fins) protrude from the ends of the shaft. The fins can point inwards to form an arrow "head" or outwards to form an arrow "tail". The line segment forming the shaft of the arrow with two tails is perceived to be longer than that forming the shaft of the arrow with two heads. ==Variation in perception {{anchor|Variation in Perception}}== Research has shown that sensation of the Müller-Lyer illusion can vary. Around the turn of the 20th century, [[W. H. R. Rivers]] noted that indigenous people of the Australian [[Murray Island, Queensland|Murray Island]] were less susceptible to the Müller-Lyer illusion than were Europeans.<ref>{{cite report | vauthors = Rivers WH | date = 1901 | url = https://www.biodiversitylibrary.org/page/29464021 | title = The measurement of visual illusion | work = Report of the British Association for the Advancement of Science | page = 818 }}</ref> Rivers suggested that this difference may be because Europeans live in more rectilinear environments than the islanders.<ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Rivers WH | date = 1901 | chapter = Vision | veditors = Haddon AC | title = Reports of the Cambridge Anthropological Expedition to Torres Straits | pages = 1–132 | location = Cambridge, UK | publisher = Cambridge University Press }}</ref> Similar results were also observed by [[John W. Berry (psychologist)|John W. Berry]] in his work on [[Inuit]], urban [[Scottish people|Scots]], and the [[Temne people]] in the 1960s.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Berry JW | title = Ecology, perceptual development and the Müller-Lyer illusion | journal = British Journal of Psychology | volume = 59 | issue = 3 | pages = 205–210 | date = August 1968 | pmid = 5760069 | doi = 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1968.tb01134.x }}</ref> In 1963, Segall, Campbell and Herskovitz compared susceptibility to four different visual illusions in three population samples of Caucasians, twelve of Africans, and one from the Philippines. For the Müller-Lyer illusion, the mean fractional misperception of the length of the line segments varied from 1.4% to 20.3%. The three European-derived samples were the three most susceptible samples, while the [[San people|San]] foragers of the Kalahari desert were the least susceptible.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Segall MH, Campbell DT, Herskovits MJ | title = Cultural differences in the perception of geometric illusions | journal = Science | volume = 139 | issue = 3556 | pages = 769–771 | date = February 1963 | pmid = 13987678 | doi = 10.1126/science.139.3556.769 }}</ref> In 1965, following a debate between [[Donald T. Campbell]] and [[Melville J. Herskovits]] on whether culture can influence such basic aspects of perception such as the length of a line, they suggested that their student [[Marshall Segall]] investigate the problem. In their definitive paper of 1966, they investigated seventeen cultures and showed that people in different cultures differ substantially on how they experience the Müller-Lyer stimuli. They wrote that "European and American city dwellers have a much higher percentage of rectangularity in their environments than non-Europeans and so are more susceptible to that illusion."<ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Segall MH, Campbell DT, Herskovits MJ | title = The influence of culture on visual perception. | location = Indianapolis | publisher = Bobbs-Merrill | date = 1966 | url = https://web.mit.edu/allanmc/www/socialperception14.pdf }}</ref> They also used the word "carpentered" for the environments that Europeans mostly live in - characterized by straight lines, right angles, and square corners. These conclusions were challenged in later work by Gustav Jahoda, who compared members of an African tribe living in a traditional rural environment with members of same group living in African cities. Here, no significant difference in susceptibility to the M-L illusion was found. Subsequent work by Jahoda suggested that [[Retinal pigment epithelium|retinal pigmentation]] may have a role in the differing perceptions on this illusion,<ref> {{cite journal | title = Retinal pigmentation, illusion susceptibility and space perception | vauthors = Jahoda G | journal = [[International Journal of Psychology]] | volume = 6 | number = 3 | pages = 199–207 | year = 1971 | doi = 10.1080/00207597108246683 }}</ref> and this was verified later by Pollack (1970). It is believed now that not "carpenteredness", but the density of pigmentation in the eye is related to susceptibility to the M-L illusion. Dark-skinned people often have denser eye pigmentation.<ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Cole M, Means B | chapter = What Happens When All Other Things are Not Equal? | title = Comparative studies of how people think: An introduction. | publisher = Harvard University Press | date = 1981 | isbn = 978-0-674-15261-8 | chapter-url = https://books.google.com/books?id=thLR-f10WoMC&pg=PA42}}</ref> A later study was conducted in 1978 by Ahluwalia on children and young adults from Zambia. Subjects from rural areas were compared with subjects from urban areas. The subjects from urban areas were shown to be considerably more susceptible to the illusion, as were younger subjects.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Ahluwalia A | title = An intra-cultural investigation of susceptibility to 'perspective' and 'non-perspective' spatial illusions | journal = British Journal of Psychology | volume = 69 | issue = 2 | pages = 233–241 | date = May 1978 | pmid = 656735 | doi = 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1978.tb01653.x }}</ref> While this by no means confirms the carpentered world hypothesis as such, it provides evidence that differences in the environment can create differences in the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion, even within a given culture. Experiments have been reported suggesting that pigeons perceive the standard Müller-Lyer illusion, but not the reversed.<ref name="nakamura">{{cite journal | vauthors = Nakamura N, Fujita K, Ushitani T, Miyata H | title = Perception of the standard and the reversed Müller-Lyer figures in pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens) | journal = Journal of Comparative Psychology | volume = 120 | issue = 3 | pages = 252–261 | date = August 2006 | pmid = 16893262 | doi = 10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.252 }}</ref> Experiments on parrots have also been reported with similar results.<ref name="pepperberg">{{cite journal | vauthors = Pepperberg IM, Vicinay J, Cavanagh P | title = Processing of the Müller-Lyer illusion by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). | journal = Perception | date = May 2008 | volume = 37 | issue = 5 | pages = 765–781 | doi = 10.1068/p5898 | pmid = 18605149 |url= http://www.alexfoundation.org/pdf/irene_pdf/PerceptionAlexML.pdf |access-date=2011-07-30 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130508153406/http://www.alexfoundation.org/pdf/irene_pdf/PerceptionAlexML.pdf |archive-date= 2013-05-08 }}</ref> ==Perspective explanation== [[Image:Mueller lyer.svg|thumb|right|The Müller-Lyer effect in a non-illusion]] One possible explanation, given by [[Richard Gregory]],<ref>{{cite book | vauthors = Gregory RL |title=Eye and Brain: The Psychology of Seeing |date=2005 |publisher=Princeton University Press |location=Princeton, N.J |isbn=978-0-19-852412-0 |edition=5th}}</ref> is that the Müller-Lyer illusion occurs because the visual system learns that the "angles in" configuration corresponds to a rectilinear object, such as the convex corner of a room, which is closer, and the "angles out" configuration corresponds to an object which is far away, such as the concave corner of a room. However, in a recent report<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Howe CQ, Purves D | title = The Müller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of image-source relationships | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 102 | issue = 4 | pages = 1234–1239 | date = January 2005 | pmid = 15657142 | pmc = 544622 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.0409314102 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Catherine Howe and Dale Purves contradicted Gregory's explanation: <blockquote>Although Gregory's intuition about the empirical significance of the Müller-Lyer stimulus points in the right general direction (i.e., an explanation based on past experience with the sources of such stimuli), convex and concave corners contribute little if anything to the Müller-Lyer effect.</blockquote> Neural nets in the visual system of human beings learn how to make a very efficient interpretation of [[Three-dimensional space|3D]] scenes. That is why when somebody goes away from us, we do not perceive them as getting shorter. And when we stretch one arm and look at the two hands we do not perceive one hand smaller than the other. Visual illusions are sometimes held to show us that what we see is an image created in our brain. Our brain supposedly projects the image of the smaller hand to its correct distance in our internal 3D model. This is what is called the [[size constancy]] mechanism hypothesis. In the Müller-Lyer illusion, the visual system would in this explanation detect the depth cues, which are usually associated with 3D scenes, and incorrectly decide it is a 3D drawing. Then the size constancy mechanism would make us see an erroneous length of the object which, for a true [[Perspective (visual)|perspective]] drawing, would be farther away. In the perspective drawing in the figure, we see that in usual scenes the heuristic works quite well. The width of the rug should obviously be considered shorter than the length of the wall in the back. ==Centroid explanation== [[File:BrentanoStimuli.tif|thumb|right|The classic Müller-Lyer figures (A) and three modifications (having no shaft line) of the Brentano versions of illusory figures comprising different contextual flanks: separate dots (B), the Müller-Lyer wings (C), and arcs of a circle (D, distances between the points seem to be different)]] [[File:Sarcone’s Pulsating Star (Dynamic Müller-Lyer illusion).gif|thumb|A dynamic visual demonstration by Italian researcher [[Gianni A. Sarcone]]: the blue and black segments of the star are equal in length and always the same length, though they appear to alternately stretch and shrink.]] [[File:Müller-Lyer illusion.gif|thumb| This variant of dynamic Müller-Lyer illusion by Italian researcher [[Gianni A. Sarcone]] shows that though the collinear blue and red segments seem to oscillate up and down, they are always the same length. Nothing moves except the arrows at the endpoints of each color segments. This visual illusion also involves a dynamic "[[neon color spreading]]" effect.]] According to the so-called centroid hypothesis, judgments of distance between visual objects are strongly affected by the neural computation of the [[centroid]]s of the luminance profiles of the objects, in that the position of the centroid of an image determines its perceived location.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Whitaker D, McGraw PV, Pacey I, Barrett BT | title = Centroid analysis predicts visual localization of first- and second-order stimuli | journal = Vision Research | volume = 36 | issue = 18 | pages = 2957–2970 | date = September 1996 | pmid = 8917796 | doi = 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00031-4 }}</ref> Morgan ''et al.'', suggest that the visual procedure of centroid extraction is causally related to a spatial pooling of the positional signals evoked by the neighboring object parts.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Morgan MJ, Hole GJ, Glennerster A | title = Biases and sensitivities in geometrical illusions | journal = Vision Research | volume = 30 | issue = 11 | pages = 1793–1810 | date = 1990 | pmid = 2288091 | doi = 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90160-m }}</ref> Though the integration coarsens the positional acuity, such pooling seems to be quite biologically substantiated since it allows fast and reliable assessment of the location of the visual object as whole, irrespective of its size, the shape complexity, and illumination conditions. Concerning the Müller-Lyer and similar illusions, the pattern of neural excitation evoked by contextual flank (e.g., the Müller-Lyer wings themselves) overlaps with that caused by the stimulus terminator (e.g., the wings apex), thereby leading (due to the shift of the centroid of summed excitation) to its perceptual displacement. The crucial point in the centroid explanation regarding the positional shifts of the stimulus terminators in the direction of the centroids of contextual flanks was confirmed in psychophysical examination of illusory figures with rotating distractors.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bulatov A, Bertulis A, Mickienė L, Surkys T, Bielevičius A | title = Contextual flanks' tilting and magnitude of illusion of extent | journal = Vision Research | volume = 51 | issue = 1 | pages = 58–64 | date = January 2011 | pmid = 20932991 | doi = 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.033 }}</ref> The relative displacement of all stimulus terminators leads to misjudgment of distances between them; that is, the illusion occurs as a side effect due to necessarily low spatial resolution of the neural mechanism of assessment of the relative location of the visual objects. Besides, it was shown<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bulatov A, Bulatova N, Marma V, Kučinskas L | title = Quantitative study of asymmetry in the manifestation of the wings-in and wings-out versions of the Müller-Lyer illusion | journal = Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics | volume = 84 | issue = 2 | pages = 560–575 | date = February 2022 | pmid = 34921335 | doi = 10.3758/s13414-021-02412-z }}</ref> that well-known asymmetry in manifestation of the wings-in and wings-out modifications of the Müller-Lyer illusion can be successfully explained by supplemental effects of the [[Oppel-Kundt illusion|filled-space]] illusion. [[File:RotatingBrentanoL.gif|thumb|right|Brentano figure with the rotating Müller-Lyer wings (distractors); actually, apices of the wings (stimulus terminators) are aligned and spaced equidistantly]] == References == {{reflist|30em}} == External links == *[https://michaelbach.de/ot/sze-muelue/ Müller-Lyer Illusion] *[http://www.giannisarcone.com/Muller_lyer_illusion.html Dynamic Müller-Lyer Illusion by Gianni A. Sarcone] *[https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GIwkXtDPOuE Visual explanation of the color spreading effect in the Müller-Lyer illusion] *[https://web.archive.org/web/20060907113557/http://www.mind.duke.edu/files/sites/purves/pub/1181623288.pdf The Müller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of image–source relationships] *[http://sites.google.com/site/nakamuranoriyuki2010/home_e NAKAMURA Noriyuki (Müller-Lyer Illusion in pigeons)] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230516175229/http://sites.google.com/site/nakamuranoriyuki2010/home_e |date=2023-05-16 }} *[https://web.archive.org/web/20170208092242/http://www.rit.edu/cla/gssp400/muller/muller.html The Muller-Lyer Illusion explained by Rochester Institute of Technology] {{Optical illusions}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Muller-Lyer illusion}} [[Category:Optical illusions]]
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Templates used on this page:
Template:Anchor
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite report
(
edit
)
Template:Cs1 config
(
edit
)
Template:Optical illusions
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Webarchive
(
edit
)
Search
Search
Editing
Müller-Lyer illusion
Add topic