Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Lawrence Kohlberg
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|American psychologist (1927β1987)}} {{Infobox scientist | name = Lawrence Kohlberg | image = <!-- filename only, no "File:" or "Image:" prefix, and no enclosing [[brackets]] --> | birth_date = {{birth date|1927|10|25|mf=y}} | birth_place = [[Bronxville, New York]], US | death_date = {{death date and age|1987|1|17|1927|10|25|mf=y}} | death_place = [[Winthrop, Massachusetts]], US | field = [[Psychology]] | known_for = [[Lawrence Kohlberg's stages of moral development]] | workplaces = [[University of Chicago]]<br />[[Harvard University]] | education = [[University of Chicago]] ([[B. A.|BA]], [[PhD]]) }} '''Lawrence Kohlberg''' ({{IPAc-en|Λ|k|oΚ|l|b|ΙΛr|g}}; October 25, 1927 β January 17, 1987) was an American [[psychologist]] best known for his [[Lawrence Kohlberg's stages of moral development|theory of stages of moral development]]. He served as a professor in the Psychology Department at the [[University of Chicago]] and at the [[Harvard Graduate School of Education|Graduate School of Education]] at [[Harvard University]]. Even though it was considered unusual in his era, he decided to study the topic of moral judgment, extending [[Jean Piaget]]'s account of children's moral development from 25 years earlier.<ref name="Rest">{{cite journal|last=Rest|first=James|author2=Clark Power |author3=Mary Brabeck |title=Lawrence Kohlberg (1927β1987)|journal=American Psychologist|date=May 1988|volume=43|series=5|issue=5|pages=399β400|doi=10.1037/h0091958}}<!--|access-date=9 April 2012--></ref> In fact, it took Kohlberg five years before he was able to publish an article based on his views.<ref name="Rest"/> Kohlberg's work reflected and extended not only Piaget's findings but also the theories of philosophers [[George Herbert Mead]] and [[James Mark Baldwin]].<ref>See Kohlberg, L. (1982), "Moral development," in J.M. Broughton & D.J. Freeman-Moir (Eds.), ''The Cognitive Developmental Psychology of James Mark Baldwin: Current Theory and Research in Genetic Epistemology'', Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corp.</ref> At the same time he was creating a new field within psychology: "moral development". In an empirical study using six criteria, such as citations and recognition, Kohlberg was found to be the 30th most eminent psychologist of the 20th century.<ref>Haggbloom, S.J. et al. (2002). The 100 Most Eminent Psychologists of the 20th Century. ''Review of General Psychology''. Vol. 6, No. 2, 139β15. Haggbloom et al. combined three quantitative variables: citations in professional journals, citations in textbooks, and nominations in a survey given to members of the [[Association for Psychological Science]], with three qualitative variables (converted to quantitative scores): [[National Academy of Sciences]] (NAS) membership, American Psychological Association (APA) President and/or recipient of the APA Distinguished Scientific Contributions Award, and surname used as an eponym. Then the list was rank ordered.</ref> ==Early life and education== Lawrence Kohlberg was born in [[Bronxville, New York]].<ref>See Fowler, J.W., Snarey, J., and DeNicola, K. (1988), ''Remembrances of Lawrence Kohlberg: A compilation of the presentations given at Service of Remembrance for Lawrence Kohlberg, at Memorial Church, Harvard University, on May 20, 1987'', Atlanta, GA: Center for Research in Faith and Moral Development.</ref> He was the youngest of four children of [[Alfred Kohlberg]],<ref>Keeley, J. (1969), ''The China Lobby Man: The Story of Alfred Kohlberg'', New Rochelle, NY: Arlington House.</ref> a [[Jewish German]] entrepreneur, and of his second wife, Charlotte Albrecht, a Christian German chemist.<ref name="Snarey" /> His parents separated when he was four years old and divorced finally when he was 14. From 1933 to 1938, Lawrence and his three siblings rotated between their mother and father for six months at a time. This rotating custody of the Kohlberg children ended in 1938, when the children were allowed to choose the parent with whom they wanted to live.<ref name="Snarey" /> Kohlberg attended high school at [[Phillips Academy]] in Andover, Massachusetts, and served in the [[United States Merchant Marine|Merchant Marine]] at the end of World War II.<ref>See Kohlberg, L. (1991), "My Personal Search for Universal Morality," in L. Kuhmerker (Ed.), ''The Kohlberg Legacy for the Helping Professions'', Birmingham, AL: R.E.P. Books.</ref> He worked for a time with the [[Haganah]] on a ship smuggling Jewish refugees from Romania into Palestine through the British Blockade.<ref>Kohlberg, Laurence, "Beds for Bananas," ''The Menorah Journal'', Autumn 1948, pp. 385β399.</ref><ref>Rudolph W. Patzert, Running the Palestine Blockade, Airlife Publishing: Shrewsbury, England, 1994.</ref> Captured by the British and held at an internment camp on Cyprus, Kohlberg escaped with fellow crew members. Kohlberg was in Palestine during the fighting in 1948 to establish the state of Israel, but refused to participate and focused on nonviolent forms of activism. He also lived on a [[kibbutz]] during this time, until he was able to return to America in 1948.<ref name="Snarey" /> In the same year, he enrolled at the University of Chicago. At the time it was possible to gain credit for courses by examination, and Kohlberg earned his bachelor's degree in one year, 1948.<ref name="Detlef">{{Cite book|last=Detlef|first=Garz|title=Lawrence Kohlberg: an introduction|location=Opladen|publisher=Barbara Budrich|date=22 July 2009|isbn=978-3-86649-285-1|oclc=457146600}}</ref> He then began study for his doctoral degree in psychology, which he completed at Chicago in 1958. In 1955 while beginning his dissertation, he married Lucille Stigberg, and the couple had two sons, David and Steven. In those early years he read [[Jean Piaget|Piaget]]'s work. Kohlberg found a scholarly approach that gave a central place to the individual's reasoning in moral decision making. At the time this contrasted with the current psychological approaches of [[behaviorism]] and [[psychoanalysis]] that explained morality as simple internalization of external cultural or parental rules, through teaching using reinforcement and punishment or identification with a parental authority.<ref>Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children's orientations toward a moral order: I. Sequence in the development of moral thought. ''Vita Humana'', ''6''(1β2), 11β33.</ref> ==Career== {{more citations needed|section|date=January 2018}} Kohlberg's first academic appointment was at Yale University, as an assistant professor of psychology, 1958β1961. <ref name="Detlef" /> Kohlberg spent a year at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, in Palo Alto, California, 1961β1962, and then joined the Psychology Department of the University of Chicago as assistant, then associate professor of psychology and human development, 1962β1967. There he instituted the Child Psychology Training Program.<ref name="Rest" /> He held a visiting appointment at the Harvard Graduate School of Education, 1967β1968, and then was appointed Professor of Education and Social Psychology there, beginning 1968, where he remained until his death.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Walsh|first=Catherine|date=January 2000|title=The life and legacy of Lawrence Kohlberg|journal=Society|volume=37|issue=2|pages=36β41|doi=10.1007/bf02686189|s2cid=144739280|issn=0147-2011}}</ref> In 1969 he accepted Rebecca Shribman-Katz's invitation of the Society for Justice-Ethics-Morals (JEM) and visited Israel to study the morality of young people in that country. This was the beginning of the life-long cooperation between JEM and Kohlberg. JEM published many books in Hebrew under his supervision, merging Kohlberg's morality theory and Jewish morality and putting it into practice, in teaching justice, ethics and morals to judges, lawyers, teachers, police officers, prisoners and the young generation of Israel [https://www.nli.org.il/he/journals/NNL-Journals990020416790205171/NLI]. In 1978, Kohlberg invited Katz to participate in the conference of Law in a Free Society, which led to the research published in 1980 "Moral Education and Law-Related Education". ===Stages of moral development=== {{main|Lawrence Kohlberg's stages of moral development}} In his unpublished 1958 dissertation, Kohlberg described what are now known as [[Lawrence Kohlberg's stages of moral development|Kohlberg's stages of moral development]].<ref name="dissertation">{{cite journal | last=Kohlberg | first=Lawrence | title=The Development of Modes of Thinking and Choices in Years 10 to 16 | journal=Ph. D. Dissertation, University of Chicago | year=1958 }}</ref> These stages are planes of moral adequacy conceived to explain the development of [[moral reasoning]]. Created while studying psychology at the University of Chicago, the theory was inspired by the work of [[Jean Piaget]] and a fascination with children's reactions to [[moral dilemma]]s.<ref name="Crain">{{cite book |last=Crain |first=William C. |title=Theories of Development |edition=2Rev |url=https://archive.org/details/theoriesofdevelo0000crai |publisher=Prentice-Hall |year=1985 |isbn=0-13-913617-7 |url-access=registration }}</ref> Kohlberg proposed a form of "Socratic" moral education and reaffirmed [[John Dewey]]'s idea that development should be the aim of education.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kohlberg |first1=Lawrence |last2=Mayer |first2=Rochelle |date=Winter 1972 |title=Development as the aim of education |journal=[[Harvard Educational Review]] |volume=42 |issue=4 |pages=449β496 |doi=10.17763/haer.42.4.kj6q8743r3j00j60 |url=http://auburn.edu/~silvesb/smicha/Kohlberg&Mayer.pdf }}</ref> He also outlined how educators can influence moral development without indoctrination and how public school can be engaged in moral education consistent with the [[United States Constitution]].<ref name="Rest"/> Kohlberg's approach begins with the assumption that humans are intrinsically motivated to explore and become competent at functioning in their environments. In social development, this leads us to imitate role models we perceive as competent and to look to them for validation.<ref>"Kohlberg, L. (1969), "Stage and sequence," ''Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research'', McGraw Hill: New York.</ref> Thus our earliest childhood references on the rightness of our and others' actions are adult role models with whom we are in regular contact. Kohlberg also held that there are common patterns of social life, observed in universally occurring social institutions, such as families, peer groups, structures, and procedures for clan or society decision-making, and cooperative work for mutual defense and sustenance. Endeavoring to become competent participants in such institutions, humans in all cultures exhibit similar actions and thoughts concerning the relations of self, others, and the social world. Furthermore, the more one is prompted to have empathy for the other person, the more quickly one learns to function well in cooperative human interactions. <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rumble |first=Ann C. |last2=Van Lange |first2=Paul A. M. |last3=Parks |first3=Craig D. |date=October 2010 |title=The benefits of empathy: When empathy may sustain cooperation in social dilemmas |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.659 |journal=European Journal of Social Psychology |language=en |volume=40 |issue=5 |pages=856β866 |doi=10.1002/ejsp.659 |issn=0046-2772}}</ref> The sequence of stages of moral development thus corresponds to a sequence of progressively more inclusive social circles (family, peers, community, etc.) within which humans seek to operate competently. When those groups function well, oriented by reciprocity and mutual care and respect, growing humans adapt to larger and larger circles of justice, care, and respect. Each stage of moral cognitive development is the realization in conscious thought of the relations of justice, care, and respect exhibited in a wider circle of social relations, including narrower circles within the wider. Kohlberg's [[theory]] holds that [[moral reasoning]], which is the basis for [[ethical]] behavior, has six identifiable developmental [[constructivism (learning theory)|constructive]] stages β each more adequate at responding to moral dilemmas than the last.<ref name="higheststage" /> Kohlberg suggested that the higher stages of moral development provide the person with greater capacities/abilities in terms of decision making and so these stages allow people to handle more complex dilemmas.<ref name="Rest"/> In studying these, Kohlberg followed the development of moral judgment beyond the ages originally studied earlier by Piaget,<ref name="piaget">{{cite book | last=Piaget | first=Jean | title=The Moral Judgment of the Child | publisher=Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co. | location=London | year=1932 |isbn=0-02-925240-7 }}</ref> who also claimed that logic and morality develop through constructive stages.<ref name="higheststage">{{cite journal | last=Kohlberg | first=Lawrence | title=The Claim to Moral Adequacy of a Highest Stage of Moral Judgment | journal=Journal of Philosophy | volume=70 | year=1973 | pages=630β646 | doi=10.2307/2025030 | issue=18 | publisher=The Journal of Philosophy, Vol. 70, No. 18 | jstor=2025030 }}</ref> Expanding considerably upon this groundwork, it was determined that the process of moral development was principally concerned with [[justice]] and that its development continued throughout the [[life expectancy|life span]],<ref name="dissertation" /> even spawning dialogue of philosophical implications of such research.<ref name="philmoral">{{cite book |last=Kohlberg |first=Lawrence |title= Essays on Moral Development, Vol. I: The Philosophy of Moral Development |publisher=Harper & Row |location=San Francisco, CA |year=1981 |isbn=0-06-064760-4 }}</ref><ref name="KohlLevineHewer">{{cite book | last=Kohlberg | first=Lawrence | author-link=Lawrence Kohlberg |author2=Charles Levine |author3=Alexandra Hewer | title=Moral stages : a current formulation and a response to critics | publisher=Karger | location=Basel, NY | year=1983 |isbn=3-8055-3716-6}}</ref> His model "is based on the assumption of co-operative social organization on the basis of justice and fairness."<ref>{{cite journal | title = Some Cross-Cultural Evidence on Ethical Reasoning | journal = Journal of Business Ethics | date = May 2001 | first = Judy | last = Tsui |author2=Carolyn Windsor | volume = 31 | issue = 2 | pages = 143β150 | doi = 10.1023/A:1010727320265 | s2cid = 141929754 }}</ref> Kohlberg studied moral reasoning by presenting subjects with [[moral dilemmas]]. He would then categorize and classify the reasoning used in the responses, into one of six distinct stages, grouped into three levels: preconventional, conventional and postconventional.<ref name="istoought">{{cite book |last=Kohlberg |first=Lawrence |title= From ''Is'' to ''Ought'': How to Commit the Naturalistic Fallacy and Get Away with It in the Study of Moral Development |publisher=Academic Press |location=New York |year=1971 }}</ref><ref name="moralization">{{cite book |last=Kohlberg |first=Lawrence | editor=T. Lickona |title=Moral Development and Behavior: Theory, Research and Social Issues |publisher=Rinehart and Winston |location=Holt, NY |year=1976 |chapter= Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive-developmental approach }}</ref><ref name="measurement">{{cite book |last=Colby |first=Anne |author2=Kohlberg, L. |title=The Measurement of Moral Judgment Vol. 2: Standard Issue Scoring Manual |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1987 | isbn=0-521-24447-1}}</ref> Each level contains two stages. These stages heavily influenced others and have been utilized by others like [[James Rest]] in making the ''[[Defining Issues Test]]'' in 1979.<ref name="judgingmoralissues">{{cite book |last=Rest |first=James |author-link=James Rest |title=Development in Judging Moral Issues |year=1979 |publisher=University of Minnesota Press |isbn=0-8166-0891-1 }}</ref> === Moral education === Kohlberg is most well known among psychologists for his research in moral psychology, but among educators he is known for his applied work of moral education in schools. The three major contributions Kohlberg made to moral education were the use of Moral Exemplars, Dilemma Discussions, and Just Community Schools.<ref name="Snarey">Snarey, J. R. (2012). Lawrence Kohlberg: Moral biography, moral psychology, and moral pedagogy. In W.E. Pickren, D. A. Dewsbury, M. Wertheimer, W. E. Pickren, D. A. Dewsbury, M. Wertheimer (Eds.), ''Portraits of pioneers in developmental psychology'' (pp. 277β296). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press.</ref> Kohlberg's first method of moral education was to examine the lives of moral exemplars who practiced principled morals such as Martin Luther King, Jr., Socrates, and Abraham Lincoln. He believed that moral exemplars' words and deeds increased the moral reasoning of those who watched and listened to them.<ref name="Snarey" /> Kohlberg never tested to see if examining the lives of moral exemplars did in fact increase moral reasoning. Recent research in moral psychology has brought back the value of witnessing moral exemplars in action or learning about their stories.<ref name="Algoe-Haidt">Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: The 'other-praising' emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. ''The Journal of Positive Psychology'', ''4''(2),105β127. doi:10.1080/17439760802650519</ref> Witnessing the virtuous acts of moral exemplars may not increase moral reasoning, but it has been shown to elicit an emotion known as moral elevation that can increase an individual's desire to be a better person and even has the potential to increase prosocial and moral behavior.<ref name="Algoe-Haidt" /><ref>Aquino, K., McFerran, B., & Laven, M. (2011). Moral identity and the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of uncommon goodness. ''Journal of Personality and Social Psychology'', ''100''(4), 703β718. doi:10.1037/a0022540</ref><ref>Schnall, S., Roper, J., & Fessler, D. M. (2010). Elevation leads to altruistic behavior. ''Psychological Science'', ''21''(3), 315β320. doi:10.1177/0956797609359882</ref><ref>Silvers, J. A., & Haidt, J. (2008). Moral elevation can induce nursing. ''Emotion'', ''8''(2), 291β295. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.291</ref> Although Kohlberg's hypothesis that moral exemplars could increase moral reasoning might be unfounded, his understanding that moral exemplars have an important place in moral education has growing support. Dilemma discussions in schools was another method proposed by Kohlberg to increase moral reasoning. Unlike moral exemplars, Kohlberg tested this method by integrating moral dilemma discussion into the curricula of school classes in humanities and social studies. Results of this and other studies using similar methods found that moral discussion does increase moral reasoning and works best if the individual in question is in discussion with a person who is using reasoning that is just one stage above their own.<ref name="Snarey" /> The final method Kohlberg used for moral education was known as "just communities". In 1974, Kohlberg worked with schools to set up democracy-based programs, where both students and teachers were given one vote to decide on school policies.<ref name="Walsh" /> The purpose of these programs were to build a sense of community in schools in order to promote democratic values and increase moral reasoning. Kohlberg's idea and development of "just communities" were greatly influenced by his time living on a kibbutz as a young adult in 1948 and when he was doing longitudinal cross-cultural research of moral development at Sasa, another kibbutz.<ref>Snarey, J. R., Reimer, J., & Kohlberg, L. (1985). Development of social-moral reasoning among kibbutz adolescents: A longitudinal cross-cultural study. ''Developmental Psychology'', ''21''(1), 3β17. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.21.1.3</ref> ===Writing=== Some of Kohlberg's most important publications were collected in his ''Essays on Moral Development'', Vols. I and II, ''The Philosophy of Moral Development'' (1981) and ''The Psychology of Moral Development'' (1984), published by Harper & Row. Other works published by Kohlgainz or about Kohlberg's theories and research include ''Consensus and Controversy'', ''The Meaning and Measurement of Moral Development'', ''Lawrence Kohlberg's Approach to Moral Education'' and ''Child Psychology and Childhood Education: A Cognitive Developmental View''.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.bookfinder.com/author/lawrence-kohlberg/ |title=Lawrence Kohlberg: books by Lawrence Kohlberg @ |publisher=Bookfinder.com |access-date=2012-08-06}}</ref> ==Critiques== [[Carol Gilligan]], a fellow researcher of Kohlberg's in the studies of moral reasoning that led to Kohlberg's developmental stage theory, suggested that to make moral judgments based on optimizing concrete human relations is not necessarily a lower stage of moral judgment than to consider objective principles. Postulating that women may develop an empathy-based ethic with a different, but not lower structure than that Kohlberg had described, Gilligan wrote ''[[In a Different Voice]]'', a book that founded a new movement of [[Ethics of care|care-based ethics]] that initially found strong resonance among [[feminists]] and later achieved wider recognition. Kohlberg's response to Carol Gilligan's criticism was that he agreed with her that there is a care moral orientation that is distinct from a justice moral orientation, but he disagreed with her claim that women scored lower than men on measures of moral developmental stages because they are more inclined to use care orientation rather than a justice orientation.<ref name="Kohlberg1984" /> Kohlberg disagreed with Gilligan's position on two grounds. Firstly, many studies measuring moral development of males and females found no difference between men and women, and when differences were found, they were attributable to differences in education, work experiences, and role-taking opportunities, but not gender.<ref name="Kohlberg1984" /> Secondly, longitudinal studies of females found the same invariant sequence of moral development as previous studies that were of males only.<ref name="Kohlberg1984" /> In other words, Gilligan's criticism of Kohlberg's moral development theory was centered on differences between males and females that did not exist. Kohlberg's detailed responses to numerous critics can be read in his book ''Essays on Moral Development: Vol.II. The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and Validity of Moral Stages.'' Another criticism against Kohlberg's theory was that it focused too much on reason at the expense of other factors. One problem with Kohlberg's focus on reason was that little empirical evidence found a relationship between moral reasoning and moral behavior. Kohlberg recognized this lack of a relationship between his moral stages and moral behavior. In an attempt to understand this, he proposed two sub-stages within each stage, to explain individual differences within each stage.<ref name="Kohlberg1984">Kohlberg, L. (1984). ''Essays on Moral Development: Vol. II. The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and Validity of Moral Stages''. San Francisco, Harper & Row.</ref> He then proposed a model of the relationship between moral judgments and moral action. According to Kohlberg,<ref name="Kohlberg1984" /> an individual first interprets the situation using their moral reasoning, which is influenced by their moral stage and sub-stage. After interpretation individuals make a deontic choice and a judgment of responsibility, which are both influenced by the stage and sub-stage of the individual. If the individual does decide on a moral action and their obligation to do it, they still need the non-moral skills to carry out a moral behavior. If this model is true then it would explain why research was having a hard time finding a direct relationship between moral reason and moral behavior. Another problem with Kohlberg's emphasis on moral reasoning is growing empirical support that individuals are more likely to use intuitive "gut reactions" to make moral decisions than use reason-based thought.<ref name="Arnold">Arnold, M. L. (2000). Stage, Sequence, and Sequels: Changing Conceptions of Morality, Post-Kohlberg. ''Educational Psychology Review'', ''12''(4), 365β383.</ref> The high use of intuition directly challenges the place of reason in moral experience. This expanding of the moral domain from reason has raised questions that perhaps morality research is entering areas of inquiry that are not considered real morality, which was a concern of Kohlberg when he first started his research.<ref name="Arnold" /> Scholars such as [[Elliot Turiel]] and [[James Rest]] have responded to Kohlberg's work with their own significant contributions. ==Death== While doing cross-cultural research in [[Belize]] in 1971, Kohlberg contracted a tropical parasitic infection,<ref>{{cite book |title=Educational Learning Theories |first1=Molly |last1=Zhou |first2=David |last2=Brown |date=2023 |publisher=Dalton State University |access-date=16 June 2024 |page=5.1.1 |url=https://socialsci.libretexts.org/Bookshelves/Early_Childhood_Education/Educational_Learning_Theories_(Zhou_and_Brown)/05%3A_Theory_of_Moral_Development/5.01%3A_Introduction}}</ref> causing him extreme abdominal pain. The long-term effects of the infection and the medications took their toll, and Kohlberg's health declined as he also engaged in increasingly demanding professional work, including "Just Community" prison and school moral education programs.<ref>Power, F.C., Higgins, A., and Kohlberg, L, ''Lawrence Kohlberg's Approach to Moral Education'', New York, NY: Columbia University Press.</ref> Kohlberg experienced depression as well. On January 17, 1987, Kohlberg parked at the end of a dead end street in Winthrop, Massachusetts, across from Boston's Logan Airport. He left his wallet with identification on the front seat of his unlocked car and apparently walked into the icy Boston Harbor. His car and wallet were found within a couple of weeks, and his body was recovered some time later, with the late winter thaw, in a tidal marsh across the harbor near the end of a Logan Airport runway.<ref name="Walsh">Walsh, Catherine (2000). "The Life and Legacy of Lawrence Kohlberg", ''Society'' ''37''(2): 38β41. {{doi|10.1007/BF02686189}}</ref> After Kohlberg's body was recovered and his death confirmed, former students and colleagues published special issues of scholarly journals to commemorate his contribution to developmental psychology.<ref>See, for instance, Boyd, D. (Ed.) (October, 1988), "Special Issue in Honour of Lawrence Kohlberg," ''Journal of Moral Education'' Vol. 17, #3; Rest, J. (Ed.) (April, 1988), "Special Issue, The Legacy of Lawrence Kohlberg," ''Counseling and Values'' Vol. 32, #3; Schrader, D. (Ed.) (Spring, 1990), "The Legacy of Lawrence Kohlberg, ''New Directions for Child Development'', #47.</ref> ==See also== *[[Heinz dilemma]] ==References== {{Library resources box|by=yes|onlinebooks=no|about=yes|wikititle=lawrence kohlberg}} {{Reflist|2}} {{Human psychological development}} {{Psychology|state=uncollapsed}} {{Authority control}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Kohlberg, Lawrence}} [[Category:American social psychologists]] [[Category:American educational psychologists]] [[Category:American developmental psychologists]] [[Category:American moral psychologists]] [[Category:20th-century American psychologists]] [[Category:Harvard University faculty]] [[Category:University of Chicago faculty]] [[Category:Phillips Academy alumni]] [[Category:University of Chicago alumni]] [[Category:Haganah members]] [[Category:Jewish philosophers]] [[Category:American people of German-Jewish descent]] [[Category:People from Bronxville, New York]] [[Category:Suicides in Massachusetts]] [[Category:Suicides by drowning in the United States]] [[Category:1927 births]] [[Category:1987 suicides]] [[Category:1987 deaths]] [[Category:United States Merchant Mariners of World War II]]
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Templates used on this page:
Template:Authority control
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Doi
(
edit
)
Template:Human psychological development
(
edit
)
Template:IPAc-en
(
edit
)
Template:Infobox scientist
(
edit
)
Template:Library resources box
(
edit
)
Template:Main
(
edit
)
Template:More citations needed
(
edit
)
Template:Psychology
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Search
Search
Editing
Lawrence Kohlberg
Add topic