Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Gambler's fallacy
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Non-examples== ===Non-independent events=== The gambler's fallacy does not apply when the probability of different events is not [[statistical independence|independent]]. In such cases, the probability of future events can change based on the outcome of past events, such as the statistical [[permutation]] of events. An example is when cards are drawn from a deck without replacement. If an [[ace]] is drawn from a deck and not reinserted, the next card drawn is less likely to be an ace and more likely to be of another rank. The probability of drawing another ace, assuming that it was the first card drawn and that there are no [[Joker (playing card)|jokers]], has decreased from {{sfrac|4|52}} (7.69%) to {{sfrac|3|51}} (5.88%), while the probability for each other rank has increased from {{sfrac|4|52}} (7.69%) to {{sfrac|4|51}} (7.84%). This effect allows [[card counting]] systems to work in games such as [[blackjack]]. ===Bias=== In most illustrations of the gambler's fallacy and the reverse gambler's fallacy, the trial (e.g. flipping a coin) is assumed to be fair. In practice, this assumption may not hold. For example, if a coin is flipped 21 times, the probability of 21 heads with a fair coin is 1 in 2,097,152. Since this probability is so small, if it happens, it may well be that the coin is somehow [[Bias (statistics)|biased]] towards landing on heads, or that it is being controlled by hidden magnets, or similar.<ref name="Gardner1986">{{cite book|last=Gardner|first=Martin|title=Entertaining Mathematical Puzzles|url=https://archive.org/details/entertainingmath00gard|url-access=registration|year=1986|publisher=Courier Dover Publications|isbn=978-0-486-25211-7|pages=[https://archive.org/details/entertainingmath00gard/page/69 69]β70|access-date=2016-03-13}}</ref> In this case, the smart bet is "heads" because [[Bayesian inference]] from the [[empirical evidence]] β 21 heads in a row β suggests that the coin is likely to be biased toward heads. Bayesian inference can be used to show that when the long-run proportion of different outcomes is unknown but [[exchangeable random variables|exchangeable]] (meaning that the random process from which the outcomes are generated may be biased but is equally likely to be biased in any direction) and that previous observations demonstrate the likely direction of the bias, the outcome which has occurred the most in the observed data is the most likely to occur again.<ref>O'Neill, B.; Puza, B.D. (2004). [http://hdl.handle.net/1885/43089 "Dice have no memories but I do: A defence of the reverse gambler's belief".] Reprinted in abridged form as: {{cite journal|last1=O'Neill|first1= B. |last2=Puza |first2=B.D.|year=2005|title= In defence of the reverse gambler's belief|journal=The Mathematical Scientist|volume=30|issue=1|pages= 13β16|issn=0312-3685}}</ref> For example, if the ''[[A priori and a posteriori|a priori]]'' probability of a biased coin is say 1%, and assuming that such a biased coin would come down heads say 60% of the time, then after 21 heads the probability of a biased coin has increased to about 32%. The opening scene of the play ''[[Rosencrantz and Guildenstern Are Dead]]'' by [[Tom Stoppard]] discusses these issues as one man continually flips heads and the other considers various possible explanations. ===Changing probabilities=== If external factors are allowed to change the probability of the events, the gambler's fallacy may not hold. For example, a change in the game rules might favour one player over the other, improving his or her win percentage. Similarly, an inexperienced player's success may decrease after opposing teams learn about and play against their weaknesses. This is another example of bias.
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Gambler's fallacy
(section)
Add topic