Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Tribalism
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
== Concept evolution == Tribalism has a very adaptive effect in [[human evolution]]. Humans are social animals and ill-equipped to live on their own.<ref>{{cite book|last=Isaacs|first=Harold Robert|title=Idols of the Tribe: Group Identity and Political Change|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=0Kne87aU7D0C|year=1975|publisher=Harvard University Press|isbn=978-0-674-44315-0|page=[https://books.google.com/books?id=0Kne87aU7D0C&pg=PA43 43]}}</ref> Tribalism and social bonding help to keep individuals committed to the group, even when personal relations may fray. That keeps individuals from wandering off or joining other groups. It also leads to [[bullying]] when a tribal member is unwilling to conform to the politics of the collective.<ref>{{cite book|last=Jenks|first=Chris|title=Core Sociological Dichotomies|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=E8BPfIuAeSQC|year=1998|publisher=Sage Publications|isbn=978-1-4462-6463-8|page=[https://books.google.com/books?id=E8BPfIuAeSQC&dq=tribalism+bonding&pg=PA339 339]}}</ref> Some scholars argue that [[inclusive fitness in humans]] involves [[kin selection]] and kin altruism, in which groups of an extended family with shared genes help others with similar genes, based on their [[coefficient of relationship]] (the amount of genes they have in common). Other scholars argue that [[fictive kinship]] is common in human organizations, allowing non-kin members to collaborate in groups like [[Fraternity|fraternities]]. Socially, divisions between groups fosters specialized interactions with others, based on association: altruism (positive interactions with unrelated members), kin-selectivity (positive interactions with related members) and violence (negative interactions). Thus, groups with a strong sense of unity and identity can benefit from [[kin selection]] behaviour such as common property and shared resources. The tendencies of members to unite against an outside tribe and the ability to act violently and prejudicially against that outside tribe likely boosted the chances of survival in genocidal conflicts. Modern examples of tribal [[genocide]] rarely reflect the defining characteristics of tribes existing prior to the [[Neolithic Revolution]]; for example, small population and close-relatedness. According to a study by [[Robin Dunbar]] at the [[University of Liverpool]], social group size is determined by primate brain size.<ref>{{cite book |author=Dunbar, Robin I. M. |title=How many friends does one person need?: Dunbar's number and other evolutionary quirks |publisher=Faber and Faber |location=London |year=2010 |isbn=978-0-571-25342-5 |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/howmanyfriendsdo0000dunb |author-link=Robin Dunbar }}</ref> Dunbar's conclusion was that most human brains can really understand only an average of 150 individuals as fully developed, complex [[person|people]]. That is known as [[Dunbar's number]]. In contrast, anthropologist [[H. Russell Bernard]] and [[Peter Killworth]] have done a variety of field studies in the United States that came up with an estimated mean number of ties, 290, roughly double Dunbar's estimate. The Bernard–Killworth [[median]] of 231 is lower because of upward straggle in the distribution, but it is still appreciably larger than Dunbar's estimate.<ref name="TwoMethods">{{cite journal |last1=McCarty |first1=C. |last2=Killworth |first2=P. D. |last3=Bernard |first3=H. R. |last4=Johnsen |first4=E. |last5=Shelley |first5=G. |title=Comparing Two Methods for Estimating Network Size |journal=Human Organization |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=28–39 |year=2000 |doi= 10.17730/humo.60.1.efx5t9gjtgmga73y|url=https://www.bebr.ufl.edu/sites/default/files/Comparing%20Two%20Methods%20for%20Estimating%20Network%20Size_0.pdf}}</ref><ref>H. Russell Bernard. [http://nersp.osg.ufl.edu/~ufruss/ "Honoring Peter Killworth's contribution to social network theory."] Paper presented to the University of Southampton, 28 September 2006.</ref> [[Malcolm Gladwell]] expanded on this conclusion sociologically in his book, ''[[The Tipping Point (book)|The Tipping Point]]'', where members of one of his types, ''Connectors'', were successful by their larger-than-average number of close friendships and capacity for maintaining them, which tie together otherwise-unconnected social groups. According to such studies, then, "tribalism" is a hard-to-escape fact of human neurology simply because many human brains are not adapted to working with large populations. Once a person's limit for connection is reached, the human brain resorts to some combination of hierarchical schemes, [[stereotype]]s and other simplified models to understand so many people.{{citation needed|date=September 2017}}
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Tribalism
(section)
Add topic