Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Sunk cost
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
=== Plan continuation bias === A related phenomenon is plan continuation bias,<ref name=Squair2011>{{Cite web|url=https://criticaluncertainties.com/2011/06/26/flying-in-the-rear-view-mirror/|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150623002211/http://criticaluncertainties.com/2011/06/26/flying-in-the-rear-view-mirror/|url-status=usurped|archive-date=June 23, 2015|title=Flying in the rear view mirror|date=2011-06-26| website=Critical Uncertainties|language=en|access-date=2019-12-28}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=https://safetyrisk.net/safety-and-the-sunk-cost-fallacy/|title=Safety and The Sunk Cost Fallacy|date=2015-06-20|website=SafetyRisk.net|language=en-US|access-date=2019-12-28}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.thehumandiver.com/blog/17-cognitive-biases|title=17 Cognitive Biases which Contribute to Diving Accidents|website=www.thehumandiver.com|language=en|access-date=2019-12-28}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Winter|first1=Scott R.|last2=Rice|first2=Stephen|last3=Capps|first3=John|last4=Trombley|first4=Justin|last5=Milner|first5=Mattie N.|last6=Anania|first6=Emily C.|last7=Walters|first7=Nathan W.|last8=Baugh|first8=Bradley S.|date=2020-03-01|title=An analysis of a pilot's adherence to their personal weather minimums|journal=Safety Science|volume=123|pages=104576|doi=10.1016/j.ssci.2019.104576|s2cid=212959377 |issn=0925-7535}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.faa.gov/news/safety_briefing/2018/media/JulAug2018.pdf|title=FAA Safety Briefing β July August 2018|website=FAA}}</ref> which is recognised as a subtle [[cognitive bias]] that tends to force the continuation of a plan or course of action even in the face of changing conditions. In the field of aerospace it has been recognised as a significant causal factor in accidents, with a 2004 NASA study finding that in 9 out of the 19 accidents studied, aircrew exhibited this behavioural bias.<ref name=Squair2011 /> This is a hazard for [[Sea captain|ships' captains]] or [[aircraft pilot]]s who may stick to a planned course even when it is leading to fatal disaster and they should abort instead. A famous example is the [[Torrey Canyon oil spill]] in which a [[SS Torrey Canyon|tanker]] ran aground when its captain persisted with a risky course rather than accepting a delay.<ref name="FT">{{citation |first=Tim |last=Harford |author-link=Tim Harford |date=18 January 2019 |url=https://www.ft.com/content/9e31e990-1a6a-11e9-b93e-f4351a53f1c3 |archive-url=https://ghostarchive.org/archive/20221210/https://www.ft.com/content/9e31e990-1a6a-11e9-b93e-f4351a53f1c3 |archive-date=2022-12-10 |url-access=subscription |url-status=live |title=Brexit lessons from the wreck of the Torrey Canyon |newspaper=Financial Times}}</ref> It has been a factor in numerous air crashes and an analysis of 279 approach and landing accidents (ALAs) found that it was the fourth most common cause, occurring in 11% of cases.<ref name="FSD">{{citation |first1=Ratan |last1=Khatwa |first2=Robert |last2=Helmreich |date=November 1998 β February 1999 |url=https://flightsafety.org/fsd/fsd_nov-feb99.pdf |title=Analysis of Critical Factors During Approach and Landing in Accidents and Normal Flight |magazine=Flight Safety Digest |pages=1β77}}</ref> Another analysis of 76 accidents found that it was a contributory factor in 42% of cases.<ref name="ASW">{{citation |first1=Benjamin A. |last1=Bermin |first2=R. Key |last2=Dismukes |url=https://flightsafety.org/wp-content/uploads/2009/07/asw_dec06.pdf |title=Pressing the Approach |magazine=Aviation Safety World |pages=28β33 |date=December 2006}}</ref> There are also two predominant factors that characterise the bias. The first is an overly [[optimism|optimistic]] estimate of probability of success, possibly to reduce [[cognitive dissonance]] having made a decision. The second is that of personal responsibility: when you are personally accountable, it is difficult for you to admit that you were wrong.<ref name=Squair2011 /> Projects often suffer [[cost overrun]]s and delays due to the [[planning fallacy]] and related factors including excessive optimism, an [[unwillingness to admit failure]], [[groupthink]] and [[Loss aversion|aversion to loss]] of sunk costs.<ref name="BIT">{{cite news |author=Behavioural Insights Team |date=July 2017 |url=https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/627790/lit-review-exploration-of-behavioural-biases.pdf |title=A review of optimism bias, planning fallacy, sunk cost bias and groupthink in project delivery and organisational decision making |work=An Exploration of Behavioural Biases in Project Delivery at the Department for Transport |publisher=[[GOV.UK]]}}</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Sunk cost
(section)
Add topic