Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Müller-Lyer illusion
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Centroid explanation== [[File:BrentanoStimuli.tif|thumb|right|The classic Müller-Lyer figures (A) and three modifications (having no shaft line) of the Brentano versions of illusory figures comprising different contextual flanks: separate dots (B), the Müller-Lyer wings (C), and arcs of a circle (D, distances between the points seem to be different)]] [[File:Sarcone’s Pulsating Star (Dynamic Müller-Lyer illusion).gif|thumb|A dynamic visual demonstration by Italian researcher [[Gianni A. Sarcone]]: the blue and black segments of the star are equal in length and always the same length, though they appear to alternately stretch and shrink.]] [[File:Müller-Lyer illusion.gif|thumb| This variant of dynamic Müller-Lyer illusion by Italian researcher [[Gianni A. Sarcone]] shows that though the collinear blue and red segments seem to oscillate up and down, they are always the same length. Nothing moves except the arrows at the endpoints of each color segments. This visual illusion also involves a dynamic "[[neon color spreading]]" effect.]] According to the so-called centroid hypothesis, judgments of distance between visual objects are strongly affected by the neural computation of the [[centroid]]s of the luminance profiles of the objects, in that the position of the centroid of an image determines its perceived location.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Whitaker D, McGraw PV, Pacey I, Barrett BT | title = Centroid analysis predicts visual localization of first- and second-order stimuli | journal = Vision Research | volume = 36 | issue = 18 | pages = 2957–2970 | date = September 1996 | pmid = 8917796 | doi = 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00031-4 }}</ref> Morgan ''et al.'', suggest that the visual procedure of centroid extraction is causally related to a spatial pooling of the positional signals evoked by the neighboring object parts.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Morgan MJ, Hole GJ, Glennerster A | title = Biases and sensitivities in geometrical illusions | journal = Vision Research | volume = 30 | issue = 11 | pages = 1793–1810 | date = 1990 | pmid = 2288091 | doi = 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90160-m }}</ref> Though the integration coarsens the positional acuity, such pooling seems to be quite biologically substantiated since it allows fast and reliable assessment of the location of the visual object as whole, irrespective of its size, the shape complexity, and illumination conditions. Concerning the Müller-Lyer and similar illusions, the pattern of neural excitation evoked by contextual flank (e.g., the Müller-Lyer wings themselves) overlaps with that caused by the stimulus terminator (e.g., the wings apex), thereby leading (due to the shift of the centroid of summed excitation) to its perceptual displacement. The crucial point in the centroid explanation regarding the positional shifts of the stimulus terminators in the direction of the centroids of contextual flanks was confirmed in psychophysical examination of illusory figures with rotating distractors.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bulatov A, Bertulis A, Mickienė L, Surkys T, Bielevičius A | title = Contextual flanks' tilting and magnitude of illusion of extent | journal = Vision Research | volume = 51 | issue = 1 | pages = 58–64 | date = January 2011 | pmid = 20932991 | doi = 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.033 }}</ref> The relative displacement of all stimulus terminators leads to misjudgment of distances between them; that is, the illusion occurs as a side effect due to necessarily low spatial resolution of the neural mechanism of assessment of the relative location of the visual objects. Besides, it was shown<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bulatov A, Bulatova N, Marma V, Kučinskas L | title = Quantitative study of asymmetry in the manifestation of the wings-in and wings-out versions of the Müller-Lyer illusion | journal = Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics | volume = 84 | issue = 2 | pages = 560–575 | date = February 2022 | pmid = 34921335 | doi = 10.3758/s13414-021-02412-z }}</ref> that well-known asymmetry in manifestation of the wings-in and wings-out modifications of the Müller-Lyer illusion can be successfully explained by supplemental effects of the [[Oppel-Kundt illusion|filled-space]] illusion. [[File:RotatingBrentanoL.gif|thumb|right|Brentano figure with the rotating Müller-Lyer wings (distractors); actually, apices of the wings (stimulus terminators) are aligned and spaced equidistantly]]
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Müller-Lyer illusion
(section)
Add topic