Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Heuristic
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
===One-reason decisions=== {{See also|Optimal stopping}} One-reason decisions are [[algorithm]]s that are made of three rules: search rules, ''confirmation rules'' (stopping), and decision rules<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = Gerd | last2 = Brighton | first2 = Henry | doi = 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | title = Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences | journal = Topics in Cognitive Science | volume = 1 | issue = 1 | pages = 107–143 | year = 2009 | pmid = 25164802 | url = https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote = This stopping rule, termed a ''confirmation rule'', works well in situations where (a) the decision maker knows little about the validity of the cues, and (b) the costs of cues are rather low (Karelaia, 2006).| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F678-0 | hdl-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = G. | last2 = Gaissmaier | first2 = W. | date = 2011 | title = Heuristic Decision Making | url = https://pure.mpg.de/rest/items/item_2099042_4/component/file_2099041/content | journal = Annual Review of Psychology | volume = 62 | issue = | pages = 451–482 | doi = 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346 | pmid = 21126183 | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote ='''One-reason decisions''': a class of heuristics that bases judgments on one good reason only, ignoring other cues (e.g., take-the-best and hiatus heuristic)| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F16D-5 | hdl-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = Gerd | last2 = Brighton | first2 = Henry | doi = 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | title = Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences | journal = Topics in Cognitive Science | volume = 1 | issue = 1 | pages = 107–143 | year = 2009 | pmid = 25164802 | url = https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote = Just as there is a class of such tracking heuristics, there is a class of one-good-reason heuristics, of which take-the-best is one member. These heuristics also have three building blocks: search rules, stopping rules, and decision rules.| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F678-0 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> *{{Annotated link|Take-the-best heuristic}}<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Todd | first1 = P | last2 = Dieckmann | first2 = A | date = 2004 | title = Heuristics for Ordering Cue Search in Decision Making | url = https://www.researchgate.net/publication/221620559 | journal = Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems | volume = | issue = | pages = 13–18 | doi = | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote = TTB consists of three building blocks. (1) Search rule: Search through cues in the order of their validity, a measure of accuracy equal to the proportion of correct decisions made by a cue out of all the times that cue discriminates between pairs of options. (2) Stopping rule: Stop search as soon as one cue is found that discriminates between the two options. (3) Decision rule: Select the option to which the discriminating cue points, that is, the option that has the cue value associated with higher criterion values.}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = Gerd | date = 2008 | title = Why Heuristics Work | url = http://www.jstor.org/stable/40212224 | journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science | volume = 3 | issue = 1 | pages = 20–29 | doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00058.x | jstor = 40212224 | pmid = 26158666 | access-date = 5 May 2024 | quote =Take the best (Gigerenzer & Goldstein, 1996). Infer which of two alternatives has the higher value by (a) searching through cues in order of validity, (b) stopping the search as soon as a cue discriminates, (c) choosing the alternative this cue favors.}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = Gerd | last2 = Brighton | first2 = Henry | doi = 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | title = Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences | journal = Topics in Cognitive Science | volume = 1 | issue = 1 | pages = 107–143 | year = 2009 | pmid = 25164802 | url = https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote = Take-the-best is a member of the one-good-reason family of heuristics because of its stopping rule: Search is stopped after finding the first cue that enables an inference to be made.| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F678-0 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> *''Hiatus heuristic'': a "recency-of-last-purchase rule"<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = G. | last2 = Gaissmaier | first2 = W. | date = 2011 | title = Heuristic Decision Making | url = https://pure.mpg.de/rest/items/item_2099042_4/component/file_2099041/content | journal = Annual Review of Psychology | volume = 62 | issue = | pages = 451–482 | doi = 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346 | pmid = 21126183 | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote = Wubben & Wangenheim (2008) reported that experienced managers use a simple recency-of-last-purchase rule: 'Hiatus heuristic: If a customer has not purchased within a certain number of months (the hiatus), the customer is classified as inactive; otherwise, the customer is classified as active.'| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F16D-5 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> *{{Annotated link|Default effect}}<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = Gerd | date = 2008 | title = Why Heuristics Work | url = http://www.jstor.org/stable/40212224 | journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science | volume = 3 | issue = 1 | pages = 20–29 | doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00058.x | jstor = 40212224 | pmid = 26158666 | access-date = 5 May 2024 | quote =Default heuristic (Johnson & Goldstein, 2003). If there is a default, do nothing about it.}}</ref> *{{Annotated link|Priority heuristic}}<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = Gerd | last2 = Brighton | first2 = Henry | doi = 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | title = Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences | journal = Topics in Cognitive Science | volume = 1 | issue = 1 | pages = 107–143 | year = 2009 | pmid = 25164802 | url = https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote = The priority heuristic, a one-good-reason heuristic with no free parameters (Brandstätter, Gigerenzer, & Hertwig, 2008; Brandstätter et al., 2006) that has similar building blocks to take-the-best, has been shown to imply (not just have parameter sets that are consistent with) several of the major violations simultaneously, including the Allais paradox and the fourfold pattern (Katsikopoulos & Gigerenzer, 2008).| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F678-0 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> *''Take-the-first heuristic''<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gigerenzer | first1 = G. | last2 = Gaissmaier | first2 = W. | date = 2011 | title = Heuristic Decision Making | url = https://pure.mpg.de/rest/items/item_2099042_4/component/file_2099041/content | journal = Annual Review of Psychology | volume = 62 | issue = | pages = 451–482 | doi = 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346 | pmid = 21126183 | access-date = 6 May 2024 | quote = Johnson & Raab (2003) proposed a variant of the fluency heuristic when alternatives are sequentially retrieved rather than simultaneously perceived: 'Take-the-first heuristic: Choose the first alternative that comes to mind.'| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F16D-5 | hdl-access = free }}</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Heuristic
(section)
Add topic