Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Aggression
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Genetics== {{Main|Genetics of aggression}} In general, differences in a continuous phenotype such as aggression are likely to result from the action of a large number of genes each of small effect, which interact with each other and the environment through development and life. In a non-mammalian example of genes related to aggression, the [[fruitless]] gene in [[Drosophila melanogaster|fruit flies]] is a critical determinant of certain sexually dimorphic behaviors, and its artificial alteration can result in a reversal of stereotypically male and female patterns of aggression in fighting. However, in what was thought to be a relatively clear case, inherent complexities have been reported in deciphering the connections between interacting genes in an environmental context and a social [[phenotype]] involving multiple behavioral and sensory interactions with another organism.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1016/j.conb.2009.04.001 |pmid=19541474 |pmc=2716404 |title=Fruitless, doublesex and the genetics of social behavior in Drosophila melanogaster |journal=Current Opinion in Neurobiology |volume=19 |issue=2 |pages=200β6 |year=2009 |last1=Siwicki |first1=Kathleen K |last2=Kravitz |first2=Edward A }}</ref> In mice, candidate genes for differentiating aggression between the sexes are the Sry (sex determining region Y) gene, located on the Y chromosome and the Sts (steroid sulfatase) gene. The Sts gene encodes the steroid sulfatase enzyme, which is pivotal in the regulation of neurosteroid biosynthesis. It is expressed in both sexes, is correlated with levels of aggression among male mice, and increases dramatically in females after [[parturition]] and during [[lactation]], corresponding to the onset of maternal aggression.<ref name="Potegal, M 1996. pp. 869-880"/> At least one study has found a possible epigenetic signature (i.e., decreased methylation at a specific CpG site on the promoter region) of the serotonin receptor 5-HT3a that is associated with maternal aggression among human subjects.<ref name=pmid27720744/> Mice with experimentally elevated sensitivity to [[oxidative stress]] (through inhibition of copper-zinc superoxide dismutase, [[SOD1]] activity) were tested for aggressive behavior.<ref name="pmid25524980">{{cite journal |vauthors=Garratt M, Brooks RC |title=A genetic reduction in antioxidant function causes elevated aggression in mice |journal=J. Exp. Biol. |volume=218 |issue=Pt 2 |pages=223β7 |date=January 2015 |pmid=25524980 |doi=10.1242/jeb.112011 |doi-access=free }}</ref> Males completely deficient in [[SOD1]] were found to be more aggressive than both wild-type males and males that express 50% of this [[antioxidant]] enzyme. They were also faster to attack another male. The causal connection between SOD1 deficiency and increased aggression is not yet understood. In humans, there is good evidence that the basic human neural architecture underpinning the potential for flexible aggressive responses is influenced by genes as well as environment. In terms of variation between individual people, more than 100 [[twin studies|twin and adoption studies]] have been conducted in recent decades examining the genetic basis of aggressive behavior and related constructs such as conduct disorders. According to a [[meta-analysis]] published in 2002, approximately 40% of variation between individuals is explained by differences in genes, and 60% by differences in environment (mainly non-shared environmental influences rather than those that would be shared by being raised together). However, such studies have depended on self-report or observation by others including parents, which complicates interpretation of the results. The few laboratory-based analyses have not found significant amounts of individual variation in aggression explicable by genetic variation in the human population. Furthermore, [[genetic linkage|linkage]] and [[genetic association|association]] studies that seek to identify specific genes, for example that influence neurotransmitter or hormone levels, have generally resulted in contradictory findings characterized by failed attempts at replication. One possible factor is an allele (variant) of the [[Monoamine oxidase A|MAO-A gene]] which, in interaction with certain life events such as childhood maltreatment (which may show a [[main effect]] on its own), can influence development of brain regions such as the amygdala and as a result some types of behavioral response may be more likely. The generally unclear picture has been compared to equally difficult findings obtained in regard to other complex behavioral phenotypes.<ref>Perusse, D. & Gendreau, P. 'Genetics and the Development of Aggression' in [https://books.google.com/books/about/Developmental_origins_of_aggression.html?id=XmSfJEl2v4sC Developmental Origins of Aggression], 2005, The Guilford Press.{{page needed|date=February 2017}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1007/s10519-010-9358-9 |pmid=20364435 |pmc=2912157 |title=Harsh Discipline, Childhood Sexual Assault, and MAOA Genotype: An Investigation of Main and Interactive Effects on Diverse Clinical Externalizing Outcomes |journal=Behavior Genetics |volume=40 |issue=5 |pages=639β48 |year=2010 |last1=Derringer |first1=Jaime |last2=Krueger |first2=Robert F. |last3=Irons |first3=Daniel E. |last4=Iacono |first4=William G. }}</ref> For example, both 7R and 5R, [[Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder|ADHD]]-linked VNTR alleles of [[Dopamine receptor D4|dopamine receptor D4 gene]] are directly associated with the incidence of proactive aggression in the men with no history of ADHD.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2015.10.002 |title=Genotype and haplotype frequencies of the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism in the men with no history of ADHD, convicted of violent crimes |journal=Journal of Criminal Justice |volume=43 |issue=6 |pages=464β469 |year=2015 |last1=Cherepkova |first1=Elena V |last2=Maksimov |first2=Vladimir N |last3=Aftanas |first3=Lyubomir I |last4=Menshanov |first4=Petr N}}</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Aggression
(section)
Add topic