Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Lullaby
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Characteristics== Lullabies tend to share exaggerated melodic tendencies, including simple [[Pitch (music)|pitch]] contours, large pitch ranges, and generally higher pitch.<ref name="Doja, Albert 2014 p. 120">Doja, Albert. "Socializing Enchantment: A Socio-Anthropological Approach to Infant-Directed Singing, Music Education and Cultural Socialization" ''International Review of the Aesthetics and Sociology of Music'', Vol. 45, No. 1 (June 2014), p. 120.</ref> These clarify and convey heightened emotions, usually of love or affection. When there is [[harmony]], infants almost always prefer [[Consonance and dissonance|consonant]] [[Interval (music)|intervals]] over dissonant intervals. Furthermore, if there is a sequence of dissonant intervals in a song, an infant will usually lose interest and it becomes very difficult to regain its attention.<ref>Trainor, Laurel J., Tsang, Christine D., Cheung, Vivian H.W. "Preference For Sensory Consonance in 2- and 4-month-Old Infants." ''Musical Perception'', Vol. 20, No. 2 (Winter 2002), pp. 187β194.</ref> To reflect this, most lullabies contain primarily consonant intervals. [[Tonality|Tonally]], most lullabies are simple, often merely alternating [[Tonic (music)|tonic]] and [[Dominant (music)|dominant]] harmonies. In addition to pitch tendencies, lullabies share several structural similarities. The most frequent tendencies are intermittent repetitions and long pauses between sections.<ref name="Mitterschiffthaler, M. T. 2007">Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., Fu, C. H.Y., Dalton, J. A., Andrew, C. M. and Williams, S. C.R. "A functional MRI study of happy and sad affective states induced by classical music" ''Human Brain Mapping'', Vol. 28 No. 11 (November 2007).</ref><ref>O'Neill, Colleen T., Trainor, Laurel J., Trehub, Sandra E. "Infants' Responsiveness to Fathers' Singing" ''Music Perception'', Vol. 18, No. 4 (Summer 2001), p. 410.</ref> This dilutes the rate of material and appeals to infants' slower capacity for processing music. Rhythmically, there are shared patterns. Lullabies are usually in triple [[Metre (music)|meter]] or 6/8 [[Time signature|time]], giving them a "characteristic swinging or rocking motion."<ref name=perry>{{cite news| url= https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/magazine-21035103 |last= Perry| first= Nina| title= The universal language of lullabies| work=BBC News|date= 20 January 2013}}</ref> This mimics the movement a baby experiences in the [[Uterus|womb]] as a mother moves. In addition, infants' preference for rhythm shares a strong connection with what they hear when they are bounced, and even their own body movements.<ref>Pouthas, V. " The development of the perception of time and temporal regulation of action in infants and children" ''Musical beginnings: Origins and development of musical competence'', (New York: Oxford University Press, 1996), pp. 115β141.</ref> The [[tempo]]s of lullabies tend to be generally slow, and the utterances are short.<ref name="Doja, Albert 2014 p. 120"/> Again, this aids in the infant's processing of the song. Lullabies almost never have instrumental accompaniments. Infants have shown a strong preference for unaccompanied lullabies over accompanied lullabies.<ref>Ilari, Beatriz and Sundara, Megha. "Music Listening Preferences in Early Life: Infants' Responses to Accompanied versus Unaccompanied Singing" ''[[Journal of Research in Music Education]]'', Vol. 56, No. 4 (January 2009), p. 356.</ref> Again, this appeals to infants' more limited ability to process information. Lullabies are often used for their soothing nature, even for non-infants. One study found lullabies to be the most successful type of music or sound for relieving stress and improving the overall psychological health of pregnant women.<ref>Chang, Mei-Yueh; Chen, Chung-Hey; Huang Kuo-Feng, "Effects of music therapy on psychological health of women during pregnancy" ''Journal of Clinical Nursing'', Vol. 17, No. 19 (October 2008), pp. 2580β2587.</ref> These characteristics tend to be consistent across cultures. It was found that adults of various cultural backgrounds could recognize and identify lullabies without knowing the cultural context of the song.<ref name="Mitterschiffthaler, M. T. 2007"/> Infants have shown a strong preferences for songs with these qualities.<ref name=Trainor96 />{{rp|19}}
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Lullaby
(section)
Add topic