Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
American Sign Language
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
== Iconicity == Common misconceptions are that signs are iconically self-explanatory, that they are a transparent imitation of what they mean, or even that they are [[pantomime]].<ref name="cxxiii">{{Harvcoltxt|Costello|2008|p=xxiii}}</ref> In fact, many signs bear no resemblance to their referent because they were originally arbitrary symbols, or their iconicity has been obscured over time.<ref name="cxxiii" /> Even so, in ASL [[iconicity]] plays a significant role; a high percentage of signs resemble their referents in some way.<ref name="l60" /> That may be because the medium of sign, three-dimensional space, naturally allows more iconicity than oral language.<ref name="cxxiii" /> In the era of the influential linguist [[Ferdinand de Saussure]], it was assumed that the mapping between form and meaning in language must be completely arbitrary.<ref name="l60">{{Harvcoltxt|Liddell|2002|p=60}}</ref> Although [[onomatopoeia]] is a clear exception, since words like "choo-choo" bear clear resemblance to the sounds that they mimic, the Saussurean approach was to treat them as marginal exceptions.<ref>{{Harvcoltxt|Liddell|2002|p=61}}</ref> ASL, with its significant inventory of iconic signs, directly challenges that theory.<ref name="l62">{{Harvcoltxt|Liddell|2002|p=62}}</ref> Research on acquisition of pronouns in ASL has shown that children do not always take advantage of the iconic properties of signs when they interpret their meaning.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Thompson|first1=Robin L.|last2=Vinson|first2=David P.|last3=Vigliocco|first3=Gabriella|date=March 2009|title=The Link Between Form and Meaning in American Sign Language: Lexical Processing Effects|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition|volume=35|issue=2|pages=550β557|doi=10.1037/a0014547|issn=0278-7393|pmc=3667647|pmid=19271866}}</ref> It has been found that when children acquire the pronoun "you", the iconicity of the point (at the child) is often confused, being treated more like a name.<ref name="petitto">{{cite journal |doi=10.1016/0010-0277(87)90034-5 |title=On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American sign language |year=1987 |last1=Petitto |first1=Laura A. |journal=Cognition |volume=27 |pages=1β52 |pmid=3691016 |issue=1|s2cid=31570908 }}</ref> That is a similar finding to research in oral languages on pronoun acquisition. It has also been found that iconicity of signs does not affect immediate memory and recall; less iconic signs are remembered just as well as highly-iconic signs.<ref>{{Harvcoltxt|Klima|Bellugi|1979|p=27}}</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
American Sign Language
(section)
Add topic