Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Addition
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
=== Childhood learning === Typically, children first master [[counting]]. When given a problem that requires that two items and three items be combined, young children model the situation with physical objects, often fingers or a drawing, and then count the total. As they gain experience, they learn or discover the strategy of "counting-on": asked to find two plus three, children count three past two, saying "three, four, ''five''" (usually ticking off fingers), and arriving at five. This strategy seems almost universal; children can easily pick it up from peers or teachers.{{sfnp|Smith|2002|p=130}} Most discover it independently. With additional experience, children learn to add more quickly by exploiting the commutativity of addition by counting up from the larger number, in this case, starting with three and counting "four, ''five''." Eventually children begin to recall certain addition facts ("[[number bond]]s"), either through experience or rote memorization. Once some facts are committed to memory, children begin to derive unknown facts from known ones. For example, a child asked to add six and seven may know that {{nowrap|1=6 + 6 = 12}} and then reason that {{nowrap|6 + 7}} is one more, or 13.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Carpenter |first=Thomas |author2=Fennema, Elizabeth|author2-link = Elizabeth Fennema |author3=Franke, Megan Loef |author4=Levi, Linda |author5=Empson, Susan|author5-link=Susan Empson |title=Children's mathematics: Cognitively guided instruction |publisher=Heinemann |year=1999 |location=Portsmouth, NH |isbn=978-0-325-00137-1 |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/childrensmathema0000unse_i5h7 }}</ref> Such derived facts can be found very quickly and most elementary school students eventually rely on a mixture of memorized and derived facts to add fluently.<ref name=Henry>{{Cite journal |last=Henry |first=Valerie J. |author2=Brown, Richard S. |title=First-grade basic facts: An investigation into teaching and learning of an accelerated, high-demand memorization standard |journal=Journal for Research in Mathematics Education |volume=39 |issue=2 |pages=153β183 |year=2008 |doi=10.2307/30034895|jstor=30034895 |doi-access=free }}</ref> Different nations introduce whole numbers and arithmetic at different ages, with many countries teaching addition in pre-school.<ref> Beckmann, S. (2014). The twenty-third ICMI study: primary mathematics study on whole numbers. International Journal of STEM Education, 1(1), 1-8. Chicago </ref> However, throughout the world, addition is taught by the end of the first year of elementary school.<ref>Schmidt, W., Houang, R., & Cogan, L. (2002). "A coherent curriculum". ''American Educator'', 26(2), 1β18.</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Addition
(section)
Add topic