Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Syntactic ambiguity
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
===Unrestricted race model=== The unrestricted race model states that analysis is affected before the introduction of ambiguity and affects which meaning is used (based on [[probability]]) before multiple analyses can be introduced. [[Van Gompel|Gompel]] and Pickering plainly refer to the unrestricted race model as a two-stage reanalysis model. Unlike constraint-based theories, only one analysis can be made at any one time. Thus, reanalysis may sometimes be necessary if information following the first analysis proves it wrong. <ref name="Gompel"/> However, the name "unrestricted race" comes directly from its properties taken from the constraint-based models. As in constraint-based theories, any source of information can support the different analyses of an ambiguous structure; thus the name. In the model, the other possible structures of an ambiguous sentence compete in a race, with the structure that is constructed fastest being used. The more such an analysis is supported, and the stronger the support is, the more likely this one will be made first.<ref>{{citation|last1=van Gompel |first1=Roger P.G. |last2=Pickering |first2=Martin J. |date=2000 |title=Unrestricted race: A new model of syntactic ambiguity resolution |citeseerx=10.1.1.165.9576 }}</ref> '''Consider the following statements:''' # "The maid of the princess who scratched herself in public was terribly humiliated." # "The son of the princess who scratched himself in public was terribly humiliated." # "The son of the princess who scratched herself in public was terribly humiliated." Research showed that people took less time to read persistently ambiguous sentences (sentence 1) than temporarily ambiguous sentences that were clarified later (sentences 2 and 3). In sentences 2 and 3, the reflexive pronouns “himself” and “herself” clarify that “who scratched” is modifying the son and the princess respectively. Thus, the readers are forced to reanalyse and their reading times will therefore rise. In sentence 1, however, the ambiguity of the reflexive pronoun “herself” fits both the maid and the princess. This means the readers do not have to reanalyse. Thus, ambiguous sentences will take a shorter time to read compared to clarified ones.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Traxler|first1=Matthew J.|last2=Pickering|first2=Martin J.|last3=Clifton|first3=Charles|date=1998-11-01|title=Adjunct Attachment Is Not a Form of Lexical Ambiguity Resolution|journal=Journal of Memory and Language|volume=39|issue=4|pages=558–592|doi=10.1006/jmla.1998.2600|issn=0749-596X}}</ref> This is called the underspecification account <ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Swets|first1=Benjamin|last2=Desmet|first2=Timothy|last3=Clifton|first3=Charles|last4=Ferreira|first4=Fernanda|date=2008-01-01|title=Underspecification of syntactic ambiguities: Evidence from self-paced reading|journal=Memory & Cognition|language=en|volume=36|issue=1|pages=201–216|doi=10.3758/MC.36.1.201|pmid=18323075|issn=1532-5946|doi-access=free}}</ref> as readers do not stick to a meaning when not provided with clarifying words. The reader understands someone scratched herself but does not seek to determine whether it was the maid or the princess. This is also known as the “good-enough” approach to understanding language.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|last1=Ferreira|first1=Fernanda|last2=Bailey|first2=Karl G.D.|last3=Ferraro|first3=Vittoria|date=February 2002|title=Good-Enough Representations in Language Comprehension|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|language=en-US|volume=11|issue=1|pages=11–15|doi=10.1111/1467-8721.00158|s2cid=4126375|issn=0963-7214}}</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Syntactic ambiguity
(section)
Add topic