Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Attention
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
===Neural correlates=== Most experiments show that one [[neural correlate]] of attention is enhanced firing. If a neuron has a different response to a stimulus when an animal is not attending to a stimulus, versus when the animal does attend to the stimulus, then the neuron's response will be enhanced even if the physical characteristics of the stimulus remain the same. In a 2007 review, Professor Eric Knudsen<ref name="Knudsen">{{cite journal | vauthors = Knudsen EI | title = Fundamental components of attention | journal = Annual Review of Neuroscience | volume = 30 | issue = 1 | pages = 57β78 | year = 2007 | pmid = 17417935 | doi = 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094256 | s2cid = 9875095 }}</ref> describes a more general [[Conceptual model|model]] which identifies four core processes of attention, with [[working memory]] at the center: * [[Working memory]] temporarily stores information for detailed analysis. * Competitive selection is the process that determines which information gains access to working memory. * Through top-down sensitivity control, higher cognitive processes can regulate signal intensity in information channels that compete for access to working memory, and thus give them an advantage in the process of competitive selection. Through top-down sensitivity control, the momentary content of working memory can influence the selection of new information, and thus mediate voluntary control of attention in a recurrent loop (endogenous attention).<ref name="Pattyn">{{cite journal | vauthors = Pattyn N, Neyt X, Henderickx D, Soetens E | title = Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: boredom or cognitive fatigue? | journal = Physiology & Behavior | volume = 93 | issue = 1β2 | pages = 369β78 | date = January 2008 | pmid = 17999934 | doi = 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.09.016 | s2cid = 9861215 }}</ref> * Bottom-up saliency filters automatically enhance the response to infrequent stimuli, or stimuli of instinctive or learned biological relevance (exogenous attention).<ref name="Pattyn"/> Neurally, at different hierarchical levels spatial maps can enhance or inhibit activity in sensory areas, and induce orienting behaviors like eye movement. * At the top of the hierarchy, the [[frontal eye fields]] (FEF) and the [[dorsolateral prefrontal cortex]] contain a retinocentric spatial map. [[Microstimulation]] in the FEF induces monkeys to make a [[saccade]] to the relevant location. Stimulation at levels too low to induce a saccade will nonetheless enhance cortical responses to stimuli located in the relevant area. * At the next lower level, a variety of spatial maps are found in the [[parietal cortex]]. In particular, the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) contains a [[saliency map]] and is interconnected both with the FEF and with sensory areas. *Exogenous attentional guidance in humans and monkeys is by a bottom-up saliency map in the [http://www.scholarpedia.org/article/Area_V1 primary visual cortex].<ref name=":1" /><ref name=":0" /> In lower [[vertebrate]]s, this saliency map is more likely in the [[superior colliculus]] (optic tectum).<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Zhaoping L | title = From the optic tectum to the primary visual cortex: migration through evolution of the saliency map for exogenous attentional guidance | journal = Current Opinion in Neurobiology | volume = 40 | pages = 94β102 | date = October 2016 | pmid = 27420378 | doi = 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.017 | s2cid = 206952820 | url = https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/1503732/ }}</ref> * Certain automatic responses that influence attention, like orienting to a highly salient stimulus, are mediated subcortically by the [[superior colliculi]]. * At the neural network level, it is thought that processes like [[lateral inhibition]] mediate the process of competitive selection. In many cases attention produces changes in the [[EEG]]. Many animals, including humans, produce [[gamma wave]]s (40β60 Hz) when focusing attention on a particular object or activity.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Kaiser J, Lutzenberger W | title = Induced gamma-band activity and human brain function | journal = The Neuroscientist | volume = 9 | issue = 6 | pages = 475β84 | date = December 2003 | pmid = 14678580 | doi = 10.1177/1073858403259137 | s2cid = 23574844 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Siegel M, Donner TH, Oostenveld R, Fries P, Engel AK | title = Neuronal synchronization along the dorsal visual pathway reflects the focus of spatial attention | journal = Neuron | volume = 60 | issue = 4 | pages = 709β19 | date = November 2008 | pmid = 19038226 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.010 | s2cid = 19010227 | doi-access = free | hdl = 2066/71012 | hdl-access = free }}</ref><ref name="ReferenceB"/><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Baldauf D, Desimone R | title = Neural mechanisms of object-based attention | journal = Science | volume = 344 | issue = 6182 | pages = 424β7 | date = April 2014 | pmid = 24763592 | doi = 10.1126/science.1247003 | bibcode = 2014Sci...344..424B | s2cid = 34728448 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Another commonly used model for the attention system has been put forth by researchers such as [[Michael Posner (psychologist)|Michael Posner]]. He divides attention into three functional components: alerting, orienting, and [[Executive system|executive attention]]<ref name="Posner"/><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Posner MI, Boies SJ |title=Components of attention|journal=Psychological Review |volume=78 |issue=5 |pages=391β408 |year=1971 |doi=10.1037/h0031333 }}</ref> that can also interact and influence each other.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Trautwein FM, Singer T, Kanske P | title = Stimulus-Driven Reorienting Impairs Executive Control of Attention: Evidence for a Common Bottleneck in Anterior Insula | journal = Cerebral Cortex | volume = 26 | issue = 11 | pages = 4136β4147 | date = August 2016 | pmid = 27550866 | pmc = 5066828 | doi = 10.1093/cercor/bhw225 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Fan J, Gu X, Guise KG, Liu X, Fossella J, Wang H, Posner MI | title = Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of attentional networks | journal = Brain and Cognition | volume = 70 | issue = 2 | pages = 209β20 | date = July 2009 | pmid = 19269079 | pmc = 2674119 | doi = 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.002 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Callejas A, LupiÑñez J, Tudela P | title = The three attentional networks: on their independence and interactions | journal = Brain and Cognition | volume = 54 | issue = 3 | pages = 225β7 | date = April 2004 | pmid = 15050779 | doi = 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.012 | s2cid = 775862 }}</ref> * Alerting is the process involved in becoming and staying attentive toward the surroundings. It appears to exist in the [[Frontal lobe|frontal]] and [[Parietal lobe|parietal]] lobes of the [[right hemisphere]], and is modulated by [[norepinephrine]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Coull JT, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Grasby PM | title = A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory | journal = Neuropsychologia | volume = 34 | issue = 11 | pages = 1085β95 | date = November 1996 | pmid = 8904746 | doi = 10.1016/0028-3932(96)00029-2 | s2cid = 25430660 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Marrocco RT, Witte EA, Davidson MC | title = Arousal systems | journal = Current Opinion in Neurobiology | volume = 4 | issue = 2 | pages = 166β70 | date = April 1994 | pmid = 7913640 | doi = 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90067-1 | s2cid = 35709525 }}</ref> * Orienting is the directing of attention to a specific stimulus. * Executive attention is used when there is a conflict between multiple attention cues. It is essentially the same as the [[Baddeley's model of working memory#Central executive|central executive]] in [[Baddeley's model of working memory]]. The [[Eriksen flanker task]] has shown that the executive control of attention may take place in the [[anterior cingulate cortex]]<ref>{{cite web | vauthors = Fan J, McCandliss BD, Flombaum JI, Thomas KM, Posner MI | date = 2001 | url = https://cognet.mit.edu/library/conferences/paper?paper_id=53066 | title = Comparing images of conflict in frontal cortex | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20150110125228/https://cognet.mit.edu/library/conferences/paper?paper_id=53066 |archive-date=2015-01-10 | work = Annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society | location = New York, NY }}</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Attention
(section)
Add topic