Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
Mind
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
== Development == === Evolution === The mind has a long evolutionary history starting with the [[Evolution of nervous systems|development of the nervous system]] and [[Evolution of the brain|the brain]].<ref>{{harvnb|Roth|2013|p=3}}</ref> While it is generally accepted today that mind is not exclusive to humans and various non-human animals have some form of mind, there is no consensus at which point exactly the mind emerged.<ref>{{harvnb|Hatfield|2013|pp=3β4}}</ref> The evolution of mind is usually explained in terms of [[natural selection]]: [[genetic variation]]s responsible for new or improved mental capacities, such as better perception or social dispositions, have an increased chance of being passed on to future generations if they are beneficial to [[Fitness (biology)|survival and reproduction]].<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Hatfield|2013|pp=4β5}} | {{harvnb|Roth|2013|pp=3β4}} }}</ref> Minimal forms of information processing are already found in the earliest forms of life 4 to 3.5 billion years ago, like the abilities of [[bacteria]] and [[Eukaryote|eukaryotic]] [[unicellular organism]]s to sense the environment, store this information, and react to it. [[Nerve cell]]s emerged with the development of [[multicellular organisms]] more than 600 million years ago as a way to process and transmit information. About 600 to 550 million years ago, an evolutionary bifurcation happened into radially symmetric organisms{{efn|They include [[cnidarians]] and [[ctenophorans]].<ref>{{harvnb|Roth|2013|pp=265β266}}</ref>}} with ring-shaped nervous systems or a [[nerve net]], like [[jellyfish]], and organisms with [[bilaterians|bilaterally symmetric bodies]], whose nervous systems tend to be more centralized. About 540 million years ago, vertebrates evolved within the group of bilaterally organized organisms. Vertebrates, like birds and [[mammal]]s, have a [[central nervous system]] including a complex brain with specialized functions. Invertebrates, like [[clam]]s and [[insect]]s, typically either have no or relatively simple brains.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Roth|2013|pp=265β266}} | {{harvnb|Erulkar|Lentz|2024|loc=Β§ Evolution and Development of the Nervous System}} | {{harvnb|Hatfield|2013|pp=6β7}} }}</ref> In the course of evolution, the brains of vertebrates tended to grow and the specialization of different brain areas tended to increase. These developments are closely related to changes in limb structures, sense organs, and living conditions with a close correspondence between the size of a brain area and the importance of its function to the organism.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Erulkar|Lentz|2024|loc=Β§ Evolution and Development of the Nervous System}} | {{harvnb|Hatfield|2013|p=13}} }}</ref> An important step in the evolution of mammals about 200 million years ago was the development of the [[neocortex]], which is responsible for many higher-order brain functions.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Aboitiz|Montiel|2007|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=N4HB0Sw-4u8C&pg=PA7 7]}} | {{harvnb|Aboitiz|Montiel|2012|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=NZ19UiDPosEC&pg=PA14 14β15]}} | {{harvnb|Finlay|Innocenti|Scheich|2013|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=9BsGCAAAQBAJ&pg=PA3 3]}} | {{harvnb|Jerison|2013|pp=7β8}} }}</ref> The size of the brain relative to the body further increased with the development of [[primate]]s, like monkeys, about 65 million years ago and later with the emergence of the first [[hominin]]s about 7β5 million years ago.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Hatfield|2013|pp=6β8}} | {{harvnb|Reyes|Sherwood|2014|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=I1kqBAAAQBAJ&pg=PA12 12]}} | {{harvnb|Wragg-Sykes|2016|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=tbbjDwAAQBAJ&pg=PA183 183]}} }}</ref> [[Anatomically modern humans]] appeared about 300,000 to 200,000 years ago.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Hatfield|2013|p=9}} | {{harvnb|Wragg-Sykes|2016|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=tbbjDwAAQBAJ&pg=PA183 183]}} | {{harvnb|Fagan|Durrani|2021|loc=[https://books.google.com/books?id=NNFKEAAAQBAJ 3. Enter Homo Sapiens (c. 300,000 Years Ago and Later)]}} }}</ref> Various theories of the evolutionary processes responsible for [[Evolution of human intelligence|human intelligence]] have been proposed. The social intelligence hypothesis says that the evolution of the human mind was triggered by the increased importance of social life and its emphasis on mental abilities associated with [[empathy]], [[knowledge transfer]], and [[meta-cognition]]. According to the ecological intelligence hypothesis, the main value of the increased mental capacities comes from their advantages in dealing with a complex physical environment through processes like behavioral flexibility, learning, and tool use. Other suggested mechanisms include the effects of a changed diet with energy-rich food and general benefits from an increased speed and efficiency of information processing.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Roth|2013|p=3}} | {{harvnb|Hatfield|2013|pp=36β43}} | {{harvnb|Mandalaywala|Fleener|Maestripieri|2014|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=ylzEBQAAQBAJ&pg=PA28 28β29]}} }}</ref> Some models propose that another major cognitive shift occurred possibly 50,000 to 40,000 years ago. Called ''[[behavioral modernity]]'', it is associated with the emergence of new or improved mental abilities, such as technological innovativeness, abstract thinking, the use of [[symbol]]s, planning, and social coordination.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|MajkiΔ|2024|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=4dPrEAAAQBAJ&pg=PA97 97β99]}} | {{harvnb|Sterelny|2011|pp=809, 811β812}} }}</ref> === Individual === Besides the development of mind in general in the course of history, [[Developmental psychology|individual human minds also develop]] in the course of their lifetime. Some of the individual changes vary from person to person as a form of learning from experience, like forming specific memories or acquiring particular behavioral patterns. Others are more universal developments as psychological stages that all or most humans go through as they pass through [[early childhood]], [[adolescence]], [[adulthood]], and [[old age]].<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|pp=171, 202, 342β343, 384}} | {{harvnb|Gross|2020|pp=171β172, 184}} | {{harvnb|Nairne|2011|pp=131β132, 240}} }}</ref> These developments cover various areas, including intellectual, sensorimotor, linguistic, emotional, social, and moral developments.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Yeomans|Arnold|2013|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=mDL1eR1VrcIC&pg=PT31 31]}} | {{harvnb|Oakley|2004|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=iSOCAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA1 1]}} | {{harvnb|Nairne|2011|pp=131β132}} }}</ref> Some factors affect the development of mind before birth, such as nutrition, maternal stress, and exposure to harmful substances like alcohol during pregnancy.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Coall|Callan|Dickins|Chisholm|2015|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=Fg3QBwAAQBAJ&pg=PA57 57β58]}} | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|pp=345β346}} | {{harvnb|Abel|2003|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=j77OEAAAQBAJ&pg=PA231 231β232]}} }}</ref> Early childhood is marked by rapid developments as [[infant]]s learn voluntary control over their bodies and interact with their environment on a basic level. Typically after about one year, this covers abilities like walking, recognizing familiar faces, and producing individual words.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|pp=342β344, 347β348, 384}} | {{harvnb|Packer|2017|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=V5NBDgAAQBAJ&pg=PA7 7β8]}} | {{harvnb|Smitsman|Corbetta|2011|loc=[https://books.google.com/books?id=8WrqZaqtzgUC&pg=PA169 Action in Infancy β Perspectives, Concepts, and Challenges]}} | {{harvnb|Nairne|2011|pp=131β132}} }}</ref> On the emotional and social levels, they develop attachments with their primary caretakers and express emotions ranging from joy and surprise to fear and anger.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Packer|2017|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=V5NBDgAAQBAJ&pg=PA7 7β8]}} | {{harvnb|Freeman|1975|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=TFjf4mUHqv4C&pg=PA114 114]}} | {{harvnb|Driscoll|Easterbrooks|2007|p=[https://books.google.com/books?id=cOPNEAAAQBAJ&pg=PA256 256]}} | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|p=384}} }}</ref> An [[Piaget's theory of cognitive development|influential theory]] by [[Jean Piaget]] divides the cognitive development of children into four stages. The sensorimotor stage from birth until two years is concerned with sensory impressions and motor activities while learning that objects remain in existence even when not observed. In the preoperational stage until seven years, children learn to interpret and use symbols in an intuitive manner. They start employing [[logical reasoning]] to physical objects in the concrete operational stage until eleven years and extend this capacity in the following formal operational stage to [[Abstract and concrete|abstract ideas]], [[Probability|probabilities]], and possibilities.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|pp=349β350}} | {{harvnb|Gross|2020|pp=566β572}} | {{harvnb|Khatoon|2012|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=Ha1KBZm9CXQC&pg=PA308 308β311]}} }}</ref> Other important processes shaping the mind in this period are [[socialization]] and [[enculturation]], at first through primary caretakers and later through peers and the schooling system.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|p=384}} | {{harvnb|Harrell|2018|pp=[https://books.google.com/books?id=lQDFDwAAQBAJ&pg=PA478 478β479]}} }}</ref> Psychological transformations during adolescence are provoked by physiological developments and by being confronted with a different social situation in the form of new expectations from others. An important factor in this period is change to the [[self-concept]], which can take the form of an [[Existential crisis#Teenage|identity crisis]]. This process typically involves developing individuality and independence from parents while at the same time seeking closeness and conformity with friends and peers. Further developments in this period include improvements to the reasoning ability and the formation of a principled moral viewpoint.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|pp=384β385}} | {{harvnb|Gross|2020|pp=619β620, 625β626}} | {{harvnb|Berman|Weems|Stickle|2006|pp=285β292}} | {{harvnb|Nairne|2011|pp=124β125, 131β132}} }}</ref> The mind also changes during adulthood but in a less rapid and pronounced manner. Reasoning and problem-solving skills improve during early and middle adulthood. Some people experience the mid-life transition as a [[midlife crisis]] involving an inner conflict about [[personal identity]], associated with anxiety, a sense of a lack of accomplishments in life, or an awareness of mortality. Intellectual faculties tend to decline in later adulthood, specifically the ability to learn complex unfamiliar tasks and later also the ability to remember. At the same time, people tend to become more inward-looking and cautious.<ref>{{multiref | {{harvnb|Bernstein|Nash|2006|p=385}} | {{harvnb|Gross|2020|pp=633β638, 664}} | {{harvnb|Nairne|2011|pp=124β125, 131β132}} }}</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
Mind
(section)
Add topic