Jump to content
Main menu
Main menu
move to sidebar
hide
Navigation
Main page
Recent changes
Random page
Help about MediaWiki
Special pages
Niidae Wiki
Search
Search
Appearance
Create account
Log in
Personal tools
Create account
Log in
Pages for logged out editors
learn more
Contributions
Talk
Editing
The Bell Curve
(section)
Page
Discussion
English
Read
Edit
View history
Tools
Tools
move to sidebar
hide
Actions
Read
Edit
View history
General
What links here
Related changes
Page information
Appearance
move to sidebar
hide
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Synopsis== {{One source|section|date=December 2019}} ''The Bell Curve'', published in 1994, was written by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray to explain the variations in intelligence in American society, warn of some consequences of that variation, and propose social policies for mitigating the worst of the consequences. The book's title comes from the bell-shaped [[normal distribution]] of [[intelligence quotient]] (IQ) scores in a population.{{citation needed|date=June 2022}} ===Introduction=== The book starts with an introduction that appraises the history of the concept of intelligence from [[Francis Galton]] to modern times. Spearman's introduction of the [[g factor (psychometrics)|general factor of intelligence]] and other early advances in research on intelligence are discussed along with a consideration of links between intelligence testing and racial politics. The 1960s are identified as the period in American history when social problems were increasingly attributed to forces outside the individual. This egalitarian ethos, Herrnstein and Murray argue, cannot accommodate biologically based individual differences.<ref name="devlin">{{cite book |title=Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to The Bell Curve |url=https://archive.org/details/intelligencegene00bern |url-access=registration |first1=Bernie |last1=Devlin |first2=Stephen E. |last2=Fienberg |first3=Daniel P. |last3=Resnick |first4=Kathryn |last4=Roeder |author4-link=Kathryn Roeder |publisher=Springer Science & Business Media |year=1997 |isbn=978-0-387-94986-4}}</ref> The introduction states six of the authors' assumptions, which they claim to be "beyond significant technical dispute":<ref name="HerrnsteinMurray2010">{{cite book |first1=Richard J. |last1=Herrnstein |first2=Charles |last2=Murray |title=The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=s4CKqxi6yWIC |date=11 May 2010 |publisher=Simon and Schuster |isbn=978-1-4391-3491-7 |pages=22–23}}</ref> # There is such a difference as a general factor of cognitive ability on which human beings differ. # All standardized tests of academic aptitude or achievement measure this general factor to some degree, but IQ tests expressly designed for that purpose measure it most accurately. # IQ scores match, to a first degree, whatever it is that people mean when they use the word intelligent, or smart in ordinary language. # IQ scores are stable, although not perfectly so, over much of a person's life. # Properly administered IQ tests are not demonstrably biased against social, economic, ethnic, or racial groups. # Cognitive ability is substantially [[Heritability|heritable]], apparently no less than 40 percent and no more than 80 percent. At the close of the introduction, the authors warn the reader against committing the [[ecological fallacy]] of inferring things about individuals based on the aggregate data presented in the book. They also assert that intelligence is just one of many valuable human attributes and one whose importance among human virtues is overrated.<ref name="devlin"/> ===Part I. The Emergence of a Cognitive Elite=== In the first part of the book Herrnstein and Murray chart how American society was transformed in the 20th century. They argue that America evolved from a society where social origin largely determined one's social status to one where cognitive ability is the leading determinant of status. The growth in college attendance, a more efficient recruitment of cognitive ability, and the sorting of cognitive ability by selective colleges are identified as important drivers of this evolution. Herrnstein and Murray propose that the cognitive elite has been produced by a more technological society which offers enough high skill jobs for those with a higher intelligence to fill. They also propose that by removing [[race (classification of human beings)|race]], [[gender]] or [[social class|class]] as criteria (via the establishment of free primary education and the prohibition of discrimination) the main criterion of success in academic and professional life is cognitive ability. Increased occupational sorting by cognitive ability is discussed. They maintain that cognitive ability is the best predictor of worker productivity.<ref name="devlin"/> Herrnstein and Murray argue that due to increasing returns to cognitive ability, a cognitive elite is being formed in America. They argue that this elite is getting richer and progressively more segregated from the rest of society.<ref name="devlin"/> ===Part II. Cognitive Classes and Social Behavior=== The second part describes how cognitive ability is related to social behaviors: high ability predicts socially desirable behavior, low ability undesirable behavior. The argument is made that group differences in social outcomes are better explained by intelligence differences rather than socioeconomic status, a perspective, the authors argue, that has been neglected in research.<ref name="devlin"/> The analyses reported in this part of the book were done using data from the [[National Longitudinal Surveys|National Longitudinal Survey of Labor Market Experience of Youth]] (NLSY), a study conducted by the [[United States Department of Labor]]'s [[Bureau of Labor Statistics]] tracking thousands of Americans starting in the 1980s. Only [[non-Hispanic white]]s are included in the analyses so as to demonstrate that the relationships between cognitive ability and social behavior are not driven by race or ethnicity.<ref name="devlin"/> Herrnstein and Murray argue that intelligence is a better predictor of individuals' outcomes than parental socioeconomic status. This argument is based on analyses where individuals' IQ scores are shown to better predict their outcomes as adults than the socioeconomic status of their parents. Such results are reported for many outcomes, including poverty, dropping out of school, unemployment, marriage, divorce, illegitimacy, welfare dependency, criminal offending, and the probability of voting in elections.<ref name="devlin"/> All participants in the NLSY took the [[Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery]] (ASVAB), a battery of ten tests taken by all who apply for entry into the armed services. (Some had taken an IQ test in high school, and the median correlation of the [[Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery#Armed Forces Qualification Test|Armed Forces Qualification Test (AFQT)]] scores and those IQ test scores was .81). Participants were later evaluated for social and economic outcomes. In general, Herrnstein and Murray argued, IQ/AFQT scores were a better predictor of life outcomes than [[social class]] background. Similarly, after statistically controlling for differences in IQ, they argued that many outcome differences between racial-ethnic groups disappeared. {| class="wikitable" |+ Economic and social correlates of IQ ! IQ || <75 || 75–90 || 90–110 || 110–125 || >125 |- | US population distribution || 5 || 20 || 50 || 20 || 5 |- | Married by age 30 || 72 || 81 || 81 || 72 || 67 |- | Out of labor force more than 1 month out of year (men) || 22 || 19 || 15 || 14 || 10 |- | Unemployed more than 1 month out of year (men) || 12 || 10 || 7 || 7 || 2 |- | Divorced in 5 years || 21 || 22 || 23 || 15 || 9 |- | % of children w/ IQ in bottom decile (mothers) || 39 || 17 || 6 || 7 || – |- | Had an [[Illegitimacy|illegitimate]] baby (mothers) || 32 || 17 || 8 || 4 || 2 |- | Lives in poverty || 30 || 16 || 6 || 3 || 2 |- | Ever [[incarcerated]] (men) || 7 || 7 || 3 || 1 || 0 |- | Chronic welfare recipient (mothers) || 31 || 17 || 8 || 2 || 0 |- | High school dropout || 55 || 35 || 6 || 0.4 || 0 |- |Scored "Yes" on "Middle Class Values Index"{{refn|group=c|According to Herrnstein & Murray the "Middle Class Values Index" was intended "to identify among the NLSY population, in their young adulthood when the index was scored, those people who are getting along with their lives in ways that fit the middle-class stereotype." To score "Yes" on the index, a NLSY subject had to meet all four of the following criteria: * Received at least a high-school diploma * Never interviewed while incarcerated * Still married to one's first spouse * ''Men only:'' In the labor force, even if not employed * ''Women only:'' Never gave birth outside of marriage Excluded from the analysis were never-married individuals who satisfied all other components of the index, and men who were not in the labor force in 1989 or 1990 due to disability or still being in school.<ref>Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp. 263–264</ref>}} || 16 || 30 || 50 || 67 || 74 |} Values are the percentage of each IQ sub-population, among non-Hispanic whites only, fitting each descriptor.<ref>Herrnstein & Murray (1994) pp. 171, 158, 163, 174, 230, 180, 132, 194, 247–248, 194, 146, 264 respectively.</ref> {{reflist|group=c}} ===Part III. The National Context=== This part of the book discusses ethnic differences in cognitive ability and social behavior. Herrnstein and Murray report that Asian Americans have a higher mean IQ than white Americans, who in turn outscore black Americans. The book argues that the black–white gap is not due to test bias, noting that IQ tests do not tend to underpredict the school or job performance of black individuals and that the gap is larger on apparently culturally neutral test items than on more culturally loaded items. The authors also note that adjusting for socioeconomic status does not eliminate the black–white IQ gap. However, they argue that the gap is narrowing.<ref name="devlin"/> According to Herrnstein and Murray, the high [[heritability of IQ]] within races does not necessarily mean that the cause of differences between races is genetic. On the other hand, they discuss lines of evidence that have been used to support the thesis that the black–white gap is at least partly genetic, such as [[Spearman's hypothesis]]. They also discuss possible environmental explanations of the gap, such as the observed generational increases in IQ, for which they coin the term [[Flynn effect]]. At the close of this discussion, they write:<ref name="devlin"/> {{blockquote|If the reader is now convinced that either the genetic or environmental explanation has won out to the exclusion of the other, we have not done a sufficiently good job of presenting one side or the other. It seems highly likely to us that both genes and environment have something to do with racial differences. What might the mix be? We are resolutely agnostic on that issue; as far as we can determine, the evidence does not yet justify an estimate.}} The authors also stress that regardless of the causes of differences, people should be treated no differently.<ref name="devlin"/> In Part III, the authors also repeat many of the analyses from Part II, but now compare whites to blacks and Hispanics in the NLSY dataset. They find that after controlling for IQ, many differences in social outcomes between races are diminished.<ref name="devlin"/> The authors discuss the possibility that high birth rates among those with lower IQs may exert a downward pressure on the national distribution of cognitive ability. They argue that immigration may also have a similar effect.<ref name="devlin"/> At the close of Part III, Herrnstein and Murray discuss the relation of IQ to social problems. Using the NLSY data, they argue that social problems are a monotonically decreasing function of IQ,<ref name="devlin"/> in other words at lower IQ scores the frequency of social problems increases. ===Living Together=== In this final chapter, the authors discuss the relevance of cognitive ability for understanding major social issues in America.<ref name="devlin"/> Evidence for experimental attempts to raise intelligence is reviewed. The authors conclude that currently there are no means to boost intelligence by more than a modest degree.<ref name="devlin"/> The authors criticize the "levelling" of general and secondary education and defend [[gifted education]]. They offer a critical overview of [[affirmative action]] policies in colleges and workplaces, arguing that their goal should be equality of opportunity rather than equal outcomes.<ref name="devlin"/> Herrnstein and Murray offer a pessimistic portrait of America's future. They predict that a cognitive elite will further isolate itself from the rest of society, while the quality of life deteriorates for those at the bottom of the cognitive scale. As an antidote to this prognosis, they offer a vision of society where differences in ability are recognized and everybody can have a valued place, stressing the role of local communities and clear moral rules that apply to everybody.<ref name="devlin"/> ===Policy recommendations=== {{Primary sources|section|date=December 2019}} Herrnstein and Murray argued the average genetic IQ of the United States is declining, owing to the tendency of the more intelligent having fewer children than the less intelligent, the generation length to be shorter for the less intelligent, and the large-scale immigration to the United States of those with low intelligence. Discussing a possible future political outcome of an intellectually stratified society, the authors stated that they "fear that a new kind of conservatism is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent—not in the social tradition of an Edmund Burke or in the economic tradition of an Adam Smith but 'conservatism' along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the menace of the slums below."<ref>p. 518.</ref> Moreover, they fear that increasing welfare will create a "custodial state" in "a high-tech and more lavish version of the Indian reservation for some substantial minority of the nation's population." They also predict increasing [[totalitarianism]]: "It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states."<ref>p. 526.</ref> The authors recommended the elimination of welfare policies which they claim encourage poor women to have babies.<ref>pp. 548–549.</ref>
Summary:
Please note that all contributions to Niidae Wiki may be edited, altered, or removed by other contributors. If you do not want your writing to be edited mercilessly, then do not submit it here.
You are also promising us that you wrote this yourself, or copied it from a public domain or similar free resource (see
Encyclopedia:Copyrights
for details).
Do not submit copyrighted work without permission!
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Search
Search
Editing
The Bell Curve
(section)
Add topic